All research at Great Ormond Street Hospital NHS Foundation Trust and UCL Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health is made possible by the NIHR Great Ormond Street Hospital Biomedical Research Centre. The views expressed in the publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR. The funders had no input into study design; data collection, analysis, and interpretation; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. The authors acknowledge the Centre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS), UCL Institute of Education, for the use of 1958-NCDS data and the UK Data Service for making them available. Neither CLS nor the UK Data Service bear any responsibility for the analysis or interpretation of these data. We thank Elise Chartrand, research assistant, for her technical support.
MCG holds a Canada Research Chair (Tier 2) and is supported by the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP). IOM holds a Canada Research Chair (Tier 2). LA is the Mental Health Leadership Fellow for the UK Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC).
Conflicts of Interest: None
Background: Bullying victimization has been associated with increased risk of suicide ideation and attempt throughout the lifespan, but no study has yet examined whether it translates to greater risk of death by suicide. We aimed to determine the association of bullying victimization with suicide mortality.
Methods: Participants were drawn from the 1958 British Birth Cohort, a prospective follow-up of all births in one week in Britain in 1958. We conducted logistic regressions on 14,946 participants whose mothers reported bullying victimization at 7 and 11 years with linked information on suicide deaths through the National Health Service Central Register.
Results: Fifty-five participants (48 males) had died by suicide between age 18 and 52 years. Bullying victimization was associated with suicide mortality; a one standard deviation increases in bullying victimization linked to an increased odds for suicide mortality (OR,1.29; 1.02–1.64) during adulthood. The OR attenuated by 11% after adjustment for individual (e.g.,behavioural and emotional problems) and familial characteristics (e.g.,adverse childhood experiences,1.18; 0.92–1.51). Analysis of bullying victimization frequency categories yield similar results : compared with individuals who had not been bullied, those who had been frequently bullied had an increased odds for suicide mortality (OR,1.89; 0.99,3.62).
Conclusion: Our study suggests that individuals who have been frequently bullied have a small increased risk of dying by suicide, when no other risk factors is considered. Suicide prevention might start in childhood, with bullying included in a range of inter-correlated vulnerabilities encompassing behavioural and emotional difficulties and adverse experiences within the family.
Suicide is one of the leading causes of death across the life-course, with 700,000 deaths each year around the world (Glenn et al., 2020; Naghavi, 2019; Turecki & Brent, 2016; World Health Organization, 2021). Every suicide is a tragedy affecting not only the person who died by suicide, but also networks of people who knew the deceased and who might be in need of support (Cerel et al., 2019).
While suicide is a multifactorial phenomenon involving complex interactions between a range of risk and protective factors (Turecki & Brent, 2016), one factor that has attracted considerable public attention recently is peer and bullying victimization (Moreno, Gower, Brittain, & Vaillancourt, 2019). Peer and bullying victimization are the experience of being the target of the aggressive behaviour by others from the same age group (Finkelhor, Turner, & Hamby, 2012; Hawker & Boulton, 2000). It can include being pushed or hit, being called names, and made fun of, or being excluded from a group. Bullying is a specific form of peer victimization which is characterized by an imbalance of power between the perpetrator and the victim, as well as repetition of the acts (Olweus, 1993). While children from diverse cultural and geographic backgrounds are exposed to peer and bullying victimization (World Health Organisation, 2012), not all children are equally likely to be exposed to such experiences. Vulnerable children such as those with lower IQ or disruptive behaviours (Oncioiu et al., 2020), those raised in an abusive home (Lereya, Samara, & Wolke, 2013) and/or who carry a range of genetic vulnerabilities related to these individual characteristics (Schoeler et al., 2019) are more likely to become the victims of their peers.
Prior systematic reviews and meta-analyses and recent observational and quasi-experimental studies found that exposure to bullying victimization in childhood predicted suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in adolescence and early adulthood (Castellví et al., 2017; Geoffroy, Boivin, et al., 2018; Geoffroy et al., 2016; Herba et al., 2008; Holt et al., 2015; John et al., 2018; Moore et al., 2017; O'Reilly et al., 2021; Schoeler, Duncan, Cecil, Ploubidis, & Pingault, 2018). A previous study from the 1958 British Birth Cohort reported that individuals who were frequently bullied had higher rates of suicidal ideation in their forties (individuals who reported that life is not worth living or who thought about suicide) than those who had not been bullied (Takizawa, Maughan, & Arseneault, 2014), suggesting that bullying victimization could act as an early adverse experience placing an individual at risk of later suicide death. Findings from the Finnish 1981 Birth Cohort Study (Klomek et al., 2009), indicated that being the victim of bullying at 8 years was associated with suicide attempts that required hospital admission (n=42) and suicide mortality (n=15, 2 females) after statistically controlling for conduct and depression symptoms at 8 years (OR=6.3 [1.5–25.9] in females and 3.8 [0.99–14.3] in males). However, as suicide attempts and deaths were combined, the specific association with suicide deaths remained unknown. While studies so far suggest that childhood bullying victimization may predispose an individual to suicide death, possibly via an association with ideation and attempt, no studies have yet examined the prospective association of childhood bullying victimization with suicide mortality.
Although suicide ideation, suicide attempts and suicide mortality share a range of common risk factors, specific risk factors have also been identified (Klonsky, Saffer, & Bryan, 2018; Mars et al., 2019). For example, early adversity, including peer bullying victimization and abusive home, was not associated with the transition to suicide attempts among individuals who previously experienced suicidal ideation (Mars et al., 2019). Insights into the mechanisms leading to suicide may be gained by establishing associations of childhood bullying victimization with suicide mortality in adulthood.
Using information on bullying victimization in childhood and suicide mortality in adulthood collected by the 1958 British Birth Cohort, we investigated whether being a victim of bullying in childhood was associated with suicide mortality in adulthood. Pre-existing vulnerabilities linked to later suicide mortality such as adverse childhood experiences within the family and emotional and behavioural problems are risk factors for exposure to bullying (Arseneault, 2018; Oncioiu et al., 2020) and they may either confound the unique association between childhood bullying victimization and suicide mortality or exacerbate suicidal risk prior to or following exposure to bullying victimization. Our analyses tested (1) the associations between childhood bullying victimization and suicide death up to mid-life; (2) whether the associations persisted after accounting for sex and a range of childhood confounders including adverse intra-familial experiences and emotional and behavioural problems; and (3) whether suicide risk associated with bullying victimization was exacerbated by adverse childhood experiences and childhood emotional and behavioural problems (Afifi et al., 2020; Fisher et al., 2012; Zhu et al., 2016).
The 1958 British Birth Cohort, also known as the National Child Development Study, is an ongoing longitudinal national birth cohort of 17,638 individuals born in 1 week of March 1958 in England, Scotland, and Wales and representing 98% of all births in Britain in that week. Details about the cohort, including study design and response rates can be found elsewhere (Power & Elliott, 2006).
Of the 17,638 participants in the birth survey of 1958, we excluded 1,168 participants who had emigrated permanently from Britain (up to 2009, as information was not available thereafter) because the National Health Service Central Register is not notified of deaths of emigrants. We further excluded 1,524 participants who had died from causes other than suicide by 2012 (most deaths had occurred within the first year of life), leaving 14,946 participants for the analyses on suicide deaths.
Ethical approval was given by the South-East Multicentre Research Ethics Committee and written consent was obtained from all participants.
When participants were 7 and 11 years, mothers were asked about the frequency their child had been bullied by other children: ‘never’, ‘sometimes’ or ‘frequently’. We created a continuous scale by summing participants’ scores across the two time points to reflect the frequency of bullying victimization at both ages. We transformed the scale into Z-scores to ease interpretation (ranging from -.72 to 3.49 SD). While the continuous bullying victimization frequency score was used as exposure in our main analyses, we also used a categorical variable for consistency across studies on bullying victimization in this cohort (Brimblecombe et al., 2018; Evans-Lacko et al., 2017; Takizawa, Danese, Maughan, & Arseneault, 2015; Takizawa et al., 2014). Accordingly, we used a three-level childhood bullying victimization indicator based on responses at both time points: 0=not been bullied (never at both 7 and 11 years); 1=occasionally (sometimes at either age); and 2=frequently bullied (frequently at either age or sometimes at both ages).
Participants were linked with death records via the National Health Service Central Register (NHSCR); until 31st August 2012 (end of mortality follow-up year). Similarly to our prior publications on suicidality with this cohort (Geoffroy, Gunnell, & Power, 2014; Richard-Devantoy et al., 2019), suicides were identified using the International Classification of Diseases, ninth revision (ICD-9) codes E950– 59 (suicide) and E980–89 (undetermined intent) or tenth revision (ICD-10) codes X60–84 (suicide) and Y10–34 (undetermined intent). Suicide and death of undetermined intent were combined (Gunnell et al., 2013). We excluded pending verdicts (ICD-9 code 988.88; ICD-10 code Y33.9). Age at death by suicide was extracted from death certificates and ranged from 18 to 52 years.
A range of childhood individual and family characteristics were selected based on the literature which includes prior studies with the cohort for their associations with bullying victimization and suicide mortality (Evans-Lacko et al., 2017; Geoffroy, Gunnell, Clark, & Power, 2018; Geoffroy et al., 2014; Richard-Devantoy et al., 2019; Takizawa et al., 2015; Takizawa et al., 2014).
Individual factors: Childhood emotional and behavioural problems (e.g., miserable, resentful/ aggressive) were reported at 7 years by the teachers via the 146-item Bristol Social Adjustment Guide (BSAG) (Stott, 1969). Childhood cognitive abilities were assessed via tests
of reading (word recognition Southgate test) (Southgate, 1962) and arithmetic, consisting of 10 problems of graded difficulty administered to participants at age 7 years. The two tests were standardized for month and year of assessment and averaged to obtain a summary score.
Family factors: Childhood socioeconomic disadvantage was identified from information on father’s social class at birth (grouped as class I or II (professional/managerial), IIINM (skilled non-manual), IIIM (skilled manual) and IV and V (semi-unskilled manual, including single households)), household amenities (bathroom, indoor lavatory, hot water) and household crowding at 7 years. Maternal age at birth was obtained from medical records. Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) within the family by age 7 years were extracted following a procedure described in prior studies with this cohort (Barboza Solís et al., 2015) and identified as a set of stressful familial psychosocial conditions. After the age-7 interview with parents in the participant’s home, health visitors recorded information on whether (1) the child has ever been in public/voluntary care services or foster care; the child lived in a household where a family member (2) is in contact with probation service; (3) has mental illness/has contact with mental health service; (4) has alcohol abuse problem; (5) the child has been separated from their father or mother due to death or divorce, or separation. The child’s schoolteacher at 7 years reported (6) whether the child appears undernourished or dirty. Exposure to ACEs was identified by an affirmative response to any of the six categories.
We examined bivariate associations between each individual and familial characteristic and suicide mortality and childhood bullying. Then, we tested whether the frequency of childhood bullying victimization (continuous score) was associated with suicide mortality using logistic regressions. We estimated four models (1) unadjusted, (2) sex-adjusted, (3) additionally adjusted for individual characteristics, including childhood emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive skills; and (4) additionally adjusted for familial characteristics including family socioeconomic disadvantage, maternal age at birth and ACEs. We also tested whether associations of bullying victimization differed by childhood emotional and behavioural problems and by the total number of ACEs. To do so, we estimated two separate models where bullying and (1) emotional and behavioural problems and (2) ACEs, as well as the two-ways interactions between those variables (bullying*emotional and behavioural problems; bullying*ACEs) were used as predictors of suicide mortality. We tested whether bullying victimization was associated with age at death by suicide. We used linear regressions with age at death by suicide as the outcome and bullying victimization as the exposure. Finally, we re-estimated our models using bullying categories as the exposure variable (none, occasionally and frequently).
Missing data on bullying victimization ranged from 13.4% at 7 years to 18.3% at 11 years (e.g. 6.1% of children were missing at both times including one suicide) while for confounders the range was from 0% (sex) to 12.1% (cognitive ability). To minimize data loss, missing data on both childhood bullying victimization and confounding variables were imputed using multiple imputation chained equations (Azur, Stuart, Frangakis, & Leaf, 2011) with all variables in the models, together with participants’ mental health at 16 years, which has been shown to be a key predictor of missingness. Regression analyses were run across 100 imputed datasets. Imputed results were broadly similar to those using observed values (Supplementary Table 1); the former is presented. Statistics were conducted in SPSS version 26th.
To complement our inferential statistical approach, and because the analyses relied on very low frequency of suicide, we applied a Bayesian statistical approach (Schmalz, Biurrun Manresa, & Zhang, 2021) to quantify evidence for the alternative (H1) relative to the null (H0) hypotheses using R version 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2020) with package brms (Bürkner, 2017) and bayestestR (Makowski, Ben-Shachar, & Lüdecke, 2019). A Bayes factor (BF10) is the ratio between the evidence that supports an association of bullying victimization with suicide (H1) over the evidence supporting no association between bullying victimization and suicide (H0). BFs greater than 1 usually indicate relative evidence for H1, whereas those smaller than 1 indicate relative evidence for H0. BFs ranging between 1-3 and 3-10 suggest anecdotal and moderate evidence for H1, respectively, whereas a BF greater than 10 represents strong evidence for H1 (Aczel, Palfi, & Szaszi, 2017). BFs were calculated using bridge-sampling (Gronau, Singmann, & Wagenmakers, 2020). All models were evaluated using non-informative priors [log student-t (3,0,2.5)], which are the default priors in brms.
Fifty-five participants had died from suicide (0.36%; 48 males and 7 females) by age 52 years; the median age of suicide was 41 years (range 18–52 years) for men and 44 years (range 21–49 years) for women.
Table 1 shows Pearson correlations for suicide mortality and potential confounders and Supplementary Table 2 Pearson correlations between potential confounders. Individuals who died by suicide had a pattern of greater levels of individual and familial vulnerabilities than their counterparts. In addition, individual and familial characteristics were correlated with childhood bullying victimization (Table 2).
Table 3 shows odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for the association of childhood bullying victimization score with suicide mortality. In unadjusted models, the bullying victimization scale was associated with increased odds of suicide mortality (OR: 1.29; 95% CI: 1.02-1.64). That is, every standard deviation increases in the bullying victimization scale was associated with a 29% increase in the odds of suicide mortality. The odds attenuated by 7% after controlling for sex (OR: 1.23; 95% CI: 0.97-1.57) and by 4% further after accounting for individual and familial vulnerabilities (1.18; 95% CI: 0.92-1.51). We found no evidence for a moderating effect of childhood emotional and behavioural problems or ACEs (P-values for interaction terms were .870 and .743 respectively). Additional analyses with age of death by suicide as the outcome indicated that bullying victimization was not associated with age at death by suicide (unadjusted ß=0.25, SE=1.15; P=0.826; fully adjusted (model 4) ß=0.434, SE=1.23; P=0.725).
Further, analyses of bullying victimization as a categorical exposure showed that frequent bullying victimization was marginally associated with an increased risk of suicide death, although confidence intervals were wide due to low prevalence of suicide (Table 4). Associations reduced after adjustment for individual and familial vulnerabilities: OR for suicide death from 1.89 (0.99,3.62) to 1.45 (0.74, 2.83).
Finally, as shown in Table 5, BFs yield moderate evidence in favor of H1 for the unadjusted model and anecdotal evidence in models adjusting for other childhood individual and familial risk factors pertaining to suicide.
Bullying and peer victimization have been associated with suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in adolescence (Geoffroy, Boivin, et al., 2018; Geoffroy et al., 2016; Gini & Espelage, 2014; Holt et al., 2015; Moore et al., 2017; Perret et al., 2020) and adulthood (Copeland, Wolke, Angold, & Costello, 2013; Klomek et al., 2009; Takizawa et al., 2014). Our study shows, in a 5-decade nationwide prospective cohort, that individuals who have been frequently bullied in childhood had an increased, albeit small, risk of dying by suicide in the adult years, when no other risk factors for suicide were considered.
To our knowledge this is the first prospective study to examine the role of childhood bullying victimization on later suicide mortality. One prior study by Klomek and al. (2009) combining information on hospital admission for suicide attempts and mortality by age 25 found an association of frequent bullying victimization. However, as suicide attempts and deaths were combined with a low number of suicides (n=15 in total; n=13 in males) the specific associations with suicide deaths were unknown. The 1958 British Birth Cohort enabled us to capture a larger group of suicide deaths. A small association of childhood bullying victimization and suicidal mortality throughout the adult years was detected, which reduced by 11% after confounding factors were controlled for. This pattern of association was consistent across both continuous and categorical measures of bullying victimization. A prior study in this cohort of the association between frequent bullying (versus not bullied) and suicidality at 45 years reported an adjusted OR of 1.66 (Takizawa et al., 2014). This estimate is comparable to the OR of 1.45 reported in our study. While both studies allowed for a similar range of confounding factors, the study by Takizawa et al. (2014) was on suicidality at age 45 years (defined as ‘thought that life is not worth living’ and ‘thought of killing yourself in the past week’) with a higher statistical power than our study of suicide deaths.
Our findings do not support the assumption that pre-existing individual (childhood emotional and behavioral problems) and familial (adversity) vulnerabilities could exacerbate suicidal risk following bullying victimization, although this finding warrants replication given the low number of suicide deaths (and related lower power to detect such effect). Our findings nevertheless point towards a cumulative effect of bullying with other forms of adversity (Afifi et al., 2020). Indeed, our study suggests that individuals exposed to the highest levels of bullying victimization were also exposed to other forms of adverse experiences in their family, hence cumulating risk factors for suicide. Further, while prior studies have shown that there is a continuity of victimization (including bullying victimization) across ages (Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2007), our measure of bullying victimization only captures two time points in childhood. Victimization may have continued into later life (i.e., re-victimization and poly-victimization). Altogether, interventions that contribute to break this vicious cycle of violence victimization may help to reduce the risk for suicide.
In this cohort, most suicides occurred in the early forties and while we did not detect an association between childhood bullying victimization and age at time of death, we can not rule out the possibility that bullying victimization might increase suicide risk factors that are in closer temporal proximity, that is, during adolescence or emerging adulthood. Of note, interpersonal conflicts and relationship difficulties are often cited as key precipitants of suicide death in psychological autopsy studies (Marttunen, Aro, & Lönnqvist, 1993) and bullying victimization was reported in about one-quarter of suicides in people younger than 20 years (Rodway et al., 2016). However, in an observational study based on 94 youth suicides in Canada, bullying had been mentioned in coroners’ verdicts for few suicides only, with more common proximal stressors mentioned being conflict with parents or romantic partners (Sinyor, Schaffer, & Cheung, 2014). Further, recent studies indicate that the new form of bullying victimization, cyberbullying, is more strongly associated, in the short term, with suicidal ideation and attempts than traditional or face-to-face bullying (Gini & Espelage, 2014; Perret et al., 2020). Studies with measures of all forms of bullying, including cyberbullying, and records of suicide deaths by early adulthood are needed to further build the evidence base for the association of bullying victimization and suicide mortality.
Our study population is large and nationally representative of British residents born in 1958, and captures for the first time, prospectively collected information on childhood bullying victimization and suicide mortality over five decades of life. However, our results should be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, our study may be underpowered to detect increasing risk of death by suicide, a rare phenomenon, among individuals who were bullied in childhood. For our bullying victimization score, CI were wide, i.e. such as 1.02 to 1.64 in the unadjusted model, suggesting that the estimated risk ranges between 2% and 64% increase in suicide mortality (or between 8% decrease to 51% increase when adjusting for all potential confounding factors). The estimation of BFs similarly provide moderate evidence for an association of bullying victimization with suicide mortality, and anecdotal evidence for rejecting the alternative hypothesis after controlling for other childhood risk factors for suicide. Second, while information on a set of key potential confounding factors was available, some factors (e.g. cognitive ability and childhood emotional and behavioural problems) were measured concurrently with bullying victimization. Because bidirectional associations exist between bullying victimization and these individual characteristics (Brunstein Klomek et al., 2019), we cannot exclude the possibility of inverse causality, and thereby to a risk of over-adjustment in our models. Accordingly, inclusion of these confounders may have reduced the power to detect an association. between bullying and mortality. Third, our bullying victimization measure was based on a single question administered to mothers (was your child ever bullied by other children), albeit at two separate ages, and no definition of bullying was provided. As parents may not be aware of all experiences of bullying victimization (Stockdale, Hangaduambo, Duys, Larson, & Sarvela, 2002), their occurrence might have been underreported by mothers, hence leading to misclassification of some children into the ‘not bullied’ category, which could have further reduced power. Despite these limitations in measurement, prior studies based on the 1958 British Birth Cohort reported consequences of bullying victimization on a range of outcomes including midlife mental health (Evans-Lacko et al., 2017; Takizawa et al., 2014), income (Brimblecombe et al., 2018; Takizawa et al., 2015) and inflammatory markers, supporting the construct validity of the bullying measure. Furthermore, reports of bullying victimization from both mothers and children have been similarly associated with children's emotional and behavioral problems (Shakoor et al., 2011). Fourth, while the cohort captures all births in a single week of March, it does not include children born in the summer or who are young for their school year, and who may be more often targeted due to their greater developmental immaturity (Whitely et al., 2019).
Although firm conclusions can’t be drawn based on this study alone, our results suggest that childhood bullying victimization may carry a small risk for suicide deaths in adulthood. However, larger population-based studies are needed to confirm these results and to clarify whether childhood bullying has a separate role beyond other childhood risk factors for death by suicide decades later. Nevertheless, suicide prevention strategies are often recommended to start in childhood, with bullying included in a constellation of co-occurring vulnerabilities encompassing behavioural and emotional difficulties, as well as adverse experiences within the family.
Table 1: Descriptive statistics (mean, SE or %, N) for suicide mortality in adulthood by childhood individual and family factors. ±
Suicide death from 18 to 52 years | |||
---|---|---|---|
Yes (N=55) | No (N=14,891) | P-valuea | |
| |||
| 87.3 (48) | 50.7 (7543) | <.001 |
| 12.28 (1.23) | 9.10 (0.07) | .010 |
| 0.01 (0.14) | -0.02 (0.01) | .797 |
| |||
| 7.78 (0.34) | 7.27 (0.02) | .137 |
| 26.00 (0.65) | 27.51(0.05) | .021 |
| 0.35 (0.09) | 0.21 (0.004) | .135 |
±Based on imputed data (n=14,946 for suicide mortality)
a Based on T-tests or Chi-Squares
Table 2: Correlations between childhood individual and family factors and bullying victimization ±
Childhood bullying victimization | |
---|---|
Individual characteristics | |
Sex (male) | .07 |
Emotional and behavioural problems | .17 |
Cognitive ability | -.17 |
Family characteristics | |
Socioeconomic disadvantage | .10 |
Maternal age at birth | -.07 |
Adverse childhood experiences | .12 |
±Based on imputed values, n= 14,946
Based on Pearson correlations
Bolded estimates are statistically significant; p<. 05.
Table 3. OR (95% CI) for the association of childhood bullying victimization with suicide mortality by mid-adulthood.±
OR (95% CI) for suicide death | P-Values | |
---|---|---|
Childhood bullying victimization | ||
Unadjusted | 1.29 (1.02, 1.64) | .032 |
Adjusted for sex | 1.23 (0.97, 1.57) | .082 |
Adjusted for individual factorsa | 1.22 (0.95, 1.55) | .112 |
Adjusted for family factorsb | 1.18 (0.92, 1.51) | .200 |
OR=odds ratio; 95%CI= 95% confidence intervals.
± Based on imputed values (n=14 946)
aThe model is adjusted for sex and individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability.
bThe model is adjusted for sex, individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability and family characteristics including socioeconomic disadvantage, maternal age and adverse childhood experiences.
Table 4. OR (95% CI) for the association of frequent and occasional childhood bullying victimization with suicide mortality by mid-adulthood.±
Suicide death† %(n) | OR (95% CI) for Suicide death± | P-Values | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Childhood bullying frequency categories | No | Yes | ||
Unadjusted | ||||
not bullied | 54.1(6073) | 43.2(19) | Ref. | |
occasionally | 27.9(3135) | 22.7(10) | 0.98 (0.47, 2.04) | .953 |
frequently | 18.0(2023) | 34.1(15) | 1.89(0.99,3.62) | .054 |
Adjusted for sex | ||||
occasionally | 0.92 (0.44, 1.91) | .816 | ||
frequently | 1.67 (0.87, 3.19) | .124 | ||
Adjusted for individual factorsa | ||||
occasionally | 0.90 (0.43, 1.89) | .783 | ||
frequently | 1.60 (0.83, 3.11) | .163 | ||
Adjusted for family factorsb | ||||
occasionally | 0.86 (0.41, 1.80) | .430 | ||
frequently | 1.45 (0.74, 2.83) | .826 |
OR=odds ratio; 95% CI=confidence intervals.
± Based on imputed values (n=14 946); † prevalence and n are based on observed data
aThe model is adjusted for sex and individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability.
bThe model is adjusted for sex, individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability and family characteristics including socioeconomic disadvantage, maternal age and adverse childhood experiences.
Table 5: Bayes factors calculated for the association between childhood bullying victimization and suicide mortality by mid-adulthood.±
BF10 | Interpretation | |
---|---|---|
Childhood bullying victimization | ||
Unadjusted | 3.73 | Moderate evidence in favor of H1 vs. H0 |
Adjusted for sex | 1.80 | Anecdotal evidence in favor of H1 vs. H0 |
Adjusted for individual factorsa | 1.35 | Anecdotal evidence in favor of H1 vs. H0 |
Adjusted for family factorsb | 0.877 | Anecdotal evidence in favor of H0 vs. H1 |
OR=odds ratio; 95%CI= 95% confidence intervals.
± Based on completed case (n=12 661)
aThe model is adjusted for sex and individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability.
bThe model is adjusted for sex, individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability and family characteristics including socioeconomic disadvantage, maternal age and adverse childhood experiences.
Aczel, B., Palfi, B., & Szaszi, B. (2017). Estimating the evidential value of significant results in psychological science. PLoS One, 12(8), e0182651. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0182651
Afifi, T. O., Taillieu, T., Salmon, S., Davila, I. G., Stewart-Tufescu, A., Fortier, J., . . . MacMillan, H. L. (2020). Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs), peer victimization, and substance use among adolescents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 106, 104504. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104504
Arseneault, L. (2018). Annual Research Review: The persistent and pervasive impact of being bullied in childhood and adolescence: implications for policy and practice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(4), 405-421. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12841
Azur, M. J., Stuart, E. A., Frangakis, C., & Leaf, P. J. (2011). Multiple imputation by chained equations: what is it and how does it work? International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 20(1), 40-49. doi:10.1002/mpr.329
Barboza Solís, C., Kelly-Irving, M., Fantin, R., Darnaudéry, M., Torrisani, J., Lang, T., & Delpierre, C. (2015). Adverse childhood experiences and physiological wear-and-tear in midlife: Findings from the 1958 British birth cohort. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(7), E738-746. doi:10.1073/pnas.1417325112
Brimblecombe, N., Evans-Lacko, S., Knapp, M., King, D., Takizawa, R., Maughan, B., & Arseneault, L. (2018). Long term economic impact associated with childhood bullying victimisation. Social Science & Medicine, 208, 134-141. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.05.014
Brunstein Klomek, A., Barzilay, S., Apter, A., Carli, V., Hoven, C. W., Sarchiapone, M., . . . Wasserman, D. (2019). Bi-directional longitudinal associations between different types of bullying victimization, suicide ideation/attempts, and depression among a large sample of European adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(2), 209-215. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12951
Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1 - 28. doi:10.18637/jss.v080.i01
Castellví, P., Miranda-Mendizábal, A., Parés-Badell, O., Almenara, J., Alonso, I., Blasco, M. J., . . . Alonso, J. (2017). Exposure to violence, a risk for suicide in youths and young adults. A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 135(3), 195-211. doi:10.1111/acps.12679
Cerel, J., Brown, M. M., Maple, M., Singleton, M., van de Venne, J., Moore, M., & Flaherty, C. (2019). How Many People Are Exposed to Suicide? Not Six. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 49(2), 529-534. doi:10.1111/sltb.12450
Copeland, W. E., Wolke, D., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2013). Adult psychiatric outcomes of bullying and being bullied by peers in childhood and adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(4), 419-426. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.504
Evans-Lacko, S., Takizawa, R., Brimblecombe, N., King, D., Knapp, M., Maughan, B., & Arseneault, L. (2017). Childhood bullying victimization is associated with use of mental health services over five decades: a longitudinal nationally representative cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 47(1), 127-135. doi:10.1017/s0033291716001719
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R. K., & Turner, H. A. (2007). Re-victimization patterns in a national longitudinal sample of children and youth. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31(5), 479-502. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.03.012
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H. A., & Hamby, S. (2012). Let's prevent peer victimization, not just bullying. Child Abuse & Neglect, 36(4), 271-274. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2011.12.001
Fisher, H. L., Moffitt, T. E., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Arseneault, L., & Caspi, A. (2012). Bullying victimisation and risk of self harm in early adolescence: longitudinal cohort study. British Medical Journal, 344, e2683. doi:10.1136/bmj.e2683
Geoffroy, M. C., Boivin, M., Arseneault, L., Renaud, J., Perret, L. C., Turecki, G., . . . Côté, S. M. (2018). Childhood trajectories of peer victimization and prediction of mental health outcomes in midadolescence: a longitudinal population-based study. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 190(2), E37-e43. doi:10.1503/cmaj.170219
Geoffroy, M. C., Boivin, M., Arseneault, L., Turecki, G., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., . . . Côté, S. M. (2016). Associations Between Peer Victimization and Suicidal Ideation and Suicide Attempt During Adolescence: Results From a Prospective Population-Based Birth Cohort. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(2), 99-105. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2015.11.010
Geoffroy, M. C., Gunnell, D., Clark, C., & Power, C. (2018). Are early-life antecedents of suicide mortality associated with psychiatric disorders and suicidal ideation in midlife? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 137(2), 116-124. doi:10.1111/acps.12844
Geoffroy, M. C., Gunnell, D., & Power, C. (2014). Prenatal and childhood antecedents of suicide: 50-year follow-up of the 1958 British Birth Cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 44(6), 1245-1256. doi:10.1017/s003329171300189x
Gini, G., & Espelage, D. L. (2014). Peer victimization, cyberbullying, and suicide risk in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Medical Association, 312(5), 545-546. doi:10.1001/jama.2014.3212
Glenn, C. R., Kleiman, E. M., Kellerman, J., Pollak, O., Cha, C. B., Esposito, E. C., . . . Boatman, A. E. (2020). Annual Research Review: A meta-analytic review of worldwide suicide rates in adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(3), 294-308. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13106
Gronau, Q. F., Singmann, H., & Wagenmakers, E. (2020). Bridgesampling: An R Package for Estimating Normalizing Constants. Journal of Statistical Software, 92(10), 1-29. doi:10.18637/jss.v092.i10
Gunnell, D., Bennewith, O., Simkin, S., Cooper, J., Klineberg, E., Rodway, C., . . . Kapur, N. (2013). Time trends in coroners' use of different verdicts for possible suicides and their impact on officially reported incidence of suicide in England: 1990-2005. Psychological Medicine, 43(7), 1415-1422. doi:10.1017/s0033291712002401
Hawker, D. S., & Boulton, M. J. (2000). Twenty years' research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: a meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(4), 441-455. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00629
Herba, C. M., Ferdinand, R. F., Stijnen, T., Veenstra, R., Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2008). Victimization and suicide ideation in the TRAILS study: specific vulnerabilities of victims. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 867-876. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01900.x
Holt, M. K., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M., Polanin, J. R., Holland, K. M., DeGue, S., Matjasko, J. L., . . . Reid, G. (2015). Bullying and suicidal ideation and behaviors: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 135(2), e496-509. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-1864
John, A., Glendenning, A. C., Marchant, A., Montgomery, P., Stewart, A., Wood, S., . . . Hawton, K. (2018). Self-Harm, Suicidal Behaviours, and Cyberbullying in Children and Young People: Systematic Review. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 20(4), e129. doi:10.2196/jmir.9044
Klomek, A. B., Sourander, A., Niemelä, S., Kumpulainen, K., Piha, J., Tamminen, T., . . . Gould, M. S. (2009). Childhood bullying behaviors as a risk for suicide attempts and completed suicides: a population-based birth cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(3), 254-261. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318196b91f
Klonsky, E. D., Saffer, B. Y., & Bryan, C. J. (2018). Ideation-to-action theories of suicide: a conceptual and empirical update. Current Opinion in Psychology, 22, 38-43. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.020
Lereya, S. T., Samara, M., & Wolke, D. (2013). Parenting behavior and the risk of becoming a victim and a bully/victim: a meta-analysis study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37(12), 1091-1108. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.03.001
Makowski, D., Ben-Shachar, M., & Lüdecke, D. (2019). BayestestR: Describing Effects and their Uncertainty, Existence and Significance within the Bayesian Framework. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(40), 1541. doi:10.21105/joss.01541
Mars, B., Heron, J., Klonsky, E. D., Moran, P., O'Connor, R. C., Tilling, K., . . . Gunnell, D. (2019). Predictors of future suicide attempt among adolescents with suicidal thoughts or non-suicidal self-harm: a population-based birth cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry, 6(4), 327-337. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(19)30030-6
Marttunen, M. J., Aro, H. M., & Lönnqvist, J. K. (1993). Precipitant stressors in adolescent suicide. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(6), 1178-1183. doi:10.1097/00004583-199311000-00010
Moore, S. E., Norman, R. E., Suetani, S., Thomas, H. J., Sly, P. D., & Scott, J. G. (2017). Consequences of bullying victimization in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. World Journal of Psychiatry, 7(1), 60-76. doi:10.5498/wjp.v7.i1.60
Moreno, M. A., Gower, A. D., Brittain, H., & Vaillancourt, T. (2019). Applying Natural Language Processing to Evaluate News Media Coverage of Bullying and Cyberbullying. Prevention Science, 20(8), 1274-1283. doi:10.1007/s11121-019-01029-x
Naghavi, M. (2019). Global, regional, and national burden of suicide mortality 1990 to 2016: systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. British Medical Journal, 364, l94. doi:10.1136/bmj.l94
O'Reilly, L. M., Pettersson, E., Quinn, P. D., Klonsky, E. D., Baldwin, J. R., Lundström, S., . . . D'Onofrio, B. M. (2021). A Co-Twin Control Study of the Association Between Bullying Victimization and Self-Harm and Suicide Attempt in Adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 69(2), 272-279. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.11.018
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Oncioiu, S. I., Orri, M., Boivin, M., Geoffroy, M. C., Arseneault, L., Brendgen, M., . . . Côté, S. M. (2020). Early Childhood Factors Associated With Peer Victimization Trajectories From 6 to 17 Years of Age. Pediatrics, 145(5). doi:10.1542/peds.2019-2654
Perret, L. C., Orri, M., Boivin, M., Ouellet-Morin, I., Denault, A.-S., Côté, S. M., . . . Geoffroy, M.-C. (2020). Cybervictimization in adolescence and its association with subsequent suicidal ideation/attempt beyond face-to-face victimization: a longitudinal population-based study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(8), 866-874. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13158
Power, C., & Elliott, J. (2006). Cohort profile: 1958 British birth cohort (National Child Development Study). International Journal of Epidemiology, 35(1), 34-41. doi:10.1093/ije/dyi183
R Core Team. (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Richard-Devantoy, S., Orri, M., Bertrand, J. A., Greenway, K. T., Turecki, G., Gunnell, D., . . . Geoffroy, M. C. (2019). Childhood cognitive skill trajectories and suicide by mid-adulthood: an investigation of the 1958 British Birth Cohort. Psychological Medicine, 1-8. doi:10.1017/s0033291719003143
Rodway, C., Tham, S. G., Ibrahim, S., Turnbull, P., Windfuhr, K., Shaw, J., . . . Appleby, L. (2016). Suicide in children and young people in England: a consecutive case series. Lancet Psychiatry, 3(8), 751-759. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(16)30094-3
Schmalz, X., Biurrun Manresa, J., & Zhang, L. (2021). What is a Bayes factor? Psychological Methods. doi:10.1037/met0000421
Schoeler, T., Choi, S. W., Dudbridge, F., Baldwin, J., Duncan, L., Cecil, C. M., . . . Pingault, J. B. (2019). Multi-Polygenic Score Approach to Identifying Individual Vulnerabilities Associated With the Risk of Exposure to Bullying. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(7), 730-738. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0310
Schoeler, T., Duncan, L., Cecil, C. M., Ploubidis, G. B., & Pingault, J. B. (2018). Quasi-experimental evidence on short- and long-term consequences of bullying victimization: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 144(12), 1229-1246. doi:10.1037/bul0000171
Shakoor, S., Jaffee, S. R., Andreou, P., Bowes, L., Ambler, A. P., Caspi, A., . . . Arseneault, L. (2011). Mothers and children as informants of bullying victimization: results from an epidemiological cohort of children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(3), 379-387. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9463-5
Sinyor, M., Schaffer, A., & Cheung, A. H. (2014). An observational study of bullying as a contributing factor in youth suicide in Toronto. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 59(12), 632-638. doi:10.1177/070674371405901204
Southgate, V. (1962). Southgate Group Reading Tests: Manual of Instructions. London: University of London Press.
Stockdale, M. S., Hangaduambo, S., Duys, D., Larson, K., & Sarvela, P. D. (2002). Rural elementary students', parents', and teachers' perceptions of bullying. American Journal of Health Behavior, 26(4), 266-277. doi:10.5993/AJHB.26.4.3
Stott, D. H. (1969). The social-adjustment of children: Manual to the Bristol Social-Adjustment Guides. London: University of London Press.
Takizawa, R., Danese, A., Maughan, B., & Arseneault, L. (2015). Bullying victimization in childhood predicts inflammation and obesity at mid-life: a five-decade birth cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 45(13), 2705-2715. doi:10.1017/s0033291715000653
Takizawa, R., Maughan, B., & Arseneault, L. (2014). Adult health outcomes of childhood bullying victimization: evidence from a five-decade longitudinal British birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(7), 777-784. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101401
Turecki, G., & Brent, D. A. (2016). Suicide and suicidal behaviour. Lancet, 387(10024), 1227-1239. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(15)00234-2
Whitely, M., Raven, M., Timimi, S., Jureidini, J., Phillimore, J., Leo, J., . . . Landman, P. (2019). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder late birthdate effect common in both high and low prescribing international jurisdictions: a systematic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 60(4), 380-391. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12991
World Health Organisation. (2012). Being bullied and bullying others. In W. H. Organisation (Ed.), Social determinants of health and well-being among young people. (pp. 191–200).
World Health Organization. (2021). Suicide worldwide in 2019: global health estimates. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/341728/9789240026643-eng.pdf?sequence=1
Zhu, J., Yu, C., Zhang, W., Bao, Z., Jiang, Y., Chen, Y., & Zhen, S. (2016). Peer victimization, deviant peer affiliation and impulsivity: Predicting adolescent problem behaviors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 58, 39-50. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.06.008.
Supplemental Table 1. OR (95% CI) for the association of childhood bullying victimization with suicide mortality by mid-adulthood, complete case±
OR (95% CI) for suicide death | P-Values | |
---|---|---|
Childhood bullying victimization | ||
Unadjusted | 1.32 (1.05, 1.67) | .017 |
Adjusted for sex | 1.27 (1.00, 1.60) | .047 |
Adjusted for individual factorsa | 1.24 (0.98, 1.58) | .073 |
Adjusted for family factorsb | 1.20 (0.94, 1.53) | .135 |
OR=odds ratio; 95%CI= 95% confidence intervals.
± Based on comleted case (n=12 661)
aThe model is adjusted for sex and individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability.
bThe model is adjusted for sex, individual characteristics including emotional and behavioural problems, and cognitive ability and family characteristics including socioeconomic disadvantage, maternal age and adverse childhood experiences.
Supplemental Table 2. Correlations between potential confounding factors.±
Sex (male) | Emotional and behavioural problems | Cognitive ability | Socioeconomic disadvantage | Maternal age at birth | Adverse childhood experiences | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex (male) | 1.00 |
|
|
|
|
|
Emotional and behavioural problems | .15*** | 1.00 |
|
|
|
|
Cognitive ability | -.01 | -.41*** | 1.00 |
|
|
|
Socioeconomic disadvantage | -.01 | .15*** | -.25*** | 1.00 |
|
|
Maternal age at birth | -.01 | -.02** | .03** | -.13*** | 1.00 |
|
Adverse childhood experiences | .01 | .22*** | -.19*** | .16*** | -.02 | 1.00 |
±Based on imputed values, n= 14,946
Based on Pearson correlations
*** Correlation is significant at the .001 level (2-tailed)
** Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)