Skip to main content
SearchLoginLogin or Signup

Fuck: The Police

A profanity policy theory proposed and tested in a pre-registered experiment to police and human resources executives in the United States.

Published onDec 02, 2023
Fuck: The Police
·

Abstract

This study focuses on police profanity, with a particular interest developing reasonable policy to regulate the use of the word "fuck." Officers employ "fuck" as a linguistic tool to accomplish a range of goals, such as establishing authority, fostering solidarity, and diffusing tension. However, “fuck” can also be used derogatorily, and negatively impact public assessments of police actions. Policy in this area is either absent, overly broad, or inappropriate to its intended use. Following brief, unstructured interviews with line and executive officers, I propose a novel policy theory of profanity, deriving target and intent. I test the theory in a pre-registered experiment administered to a sample of police and human resources executives (n=1,492), with each respondent evaluating multiple vignettes (n=5,280 observations). Results support the proposed theory and generate useful recommendations for practitioners interested in strengthening the ability of agencies to constrain professionally inappropriate use of profanity in the police workplace.

Introduction

Police officers use brutal language, which has occasionally been the target of scholarly disapproval.1 Taking a less critical view, other scholars have portrayed the profanity used by police officers as “analytically ordinary” (Sausdal, 2020) and no more than backstage (Goffman, 1959) bullshit (Frankfurt, 2009), lacking either specific intent or meaning (or both). Profanity, scholars have recently suggested, is not merely something the police use, but also describes a core police function: “they unfuck people’s problems” (Huey & Johnston, 2023, p. 1).

Beyond these theoretical considerations, empirical studies have shown that excessive use of profanity adversely affects public perception of police actions, including the use of force (Martaindale et al., 2023; Patton et al., 2017; Sharps et al., 2019). Despite these findings, contemporary police policy has been inadequate in effectively managing the use of profanity by officers. This inadequacy, I contend, is a result of a failure to precisely define the scope of the problem. Profanity serves multiple purposes in the daily lives of officers, rendering blanket prohibitions ineffective. The central question, therefore, is when should policy prohibit and sanction police profanity? And when should it recognize such incivility as situationally normal, healthy, and bond-forming (Baruch et al., 2017)?

The challenge of influencing police behavior often rests on the policies instituted by law enforcement agencies. Crafting effective policies is crucial, especially when confronting complex and sensitive issues like profanity (Alpert & Smith, 1994; Noble & Alpert, 2008). Given the potential impact of profanity on public perceptions, and range of contexts and intentions in which profanity is used, neither blanket bans nor ignoring the issue are effective or realistic. This study seeks to address this policy void by exploring the innumerable uses of the word 'fuck' by police officers with the aim of providing insights that can guide the formulation of more nuanced and effective policies on the use of profanity in professional policing contexts.

I address this gap in profanity policy in three parts. First, by canvassing the concerns of police executives, I demonstrate that there is a great deal of apprehension around using policy to address profanity, even as they generally understand it to be – in extremis – a potential problem for community relations. Here, I found that police executives were often afraid to regulate profanity on the off-chance that some profanity could be useful, normal, healthy, and even necessary to the safety of officers in some scenarios. The lack of policy guidance remains problematic, however, as officers are "highly responsive to managerial directives" (Mummolo, 2018, p. 1) and may exhibit less professionalism in the absence of guidelines, which can, in turn, undermine public trust in the police (Tyler, 2004). Second, I draw on my own experience and that of other front-line police officers (both current and former) to sketch the outward edges of the use of a specific expletive: fuck. In doing so, I find that both critics and supporters of police profanity are, in some regard, correct in their views. “Fuck” can be derogatory, demeaning, and hurtful; however, it can also be the source of much needed humor in challenging situations, as well as an affirmation of camaraderie: rather than targeting the public, the word is largely used as an outlet to “let off steam” or, conversely, as emphasis when praising colleagues and the like. In considering the executive and line-officer experiences, I propose a theory of profanity policy that focuses on the target (self, colleague, or public) and intent (neutral, positive, or derogatory) of a particular use of profanity. In the final section, I test the proposed theory, systematically varying the target and intent in a pre-registered experiment administered to a large sample of police executives and public sector human resources’ managers. I demonstrate a useful path forward for narrow, but effective, profanity policy: derogatory profanity targeting colleagues, and all forms of profanity targeting the public, are universally rated less appropriate, less professional, more harmful to public trust, and more deserving of disciplinary sanction.

Literature Review

The use of illicit, taboo, and improper words is theorized to have been with humanity for as long as language itself (Jay, 1999). While no single agreed-upon definition exists, a useable version is “the use of taboo language with the purpose of expressing the speaker’s emotional state and communicating that information to listeners.” Profanity across all forms of media has increased, and is paralleled with a rise in public usage.

In this study, I focus on the particularized use of a specific form of profanity: the word “fuck” and its derived uses and meaning. Fuck is a versatile, profane term that can be used as a verb, noun, or interjection; it can be used to express a wide range of emotions, actions, or descriptions. Depending on the context, it can be used to convey anger, annoyance, surprise, or even approval.

Fuck, therefore, is the ur-curse, “the word that has the deepest stigma of any in language” (Read, 1934, p. 264), and, until fairly recently, was considered unfit for publication. For example, it was not even cited in omnibus dictionaries until late into the 20th century. Previous scholarship proposes that, prior to the Reformation, the worst of swear words were those that took liberties with the divine. God, damn, and goddamn were all considered terrible breaches of propriety, while those dealing with intercourse and excretion had little hold on the lurid imagination, and were as common and innocuous then as the word “damn” is today. Fuck comes to us “quietly out of the mists of time” in a 1528 letter that mentions “a fuckin abbott” (McWhorter, 2021, pp. 47–48). Today, the word extends across national and language barriers, and is commonly used worldwide as an English “loan word” that replaced local language curses (Fjeld et al., 2019).

Fuck as a focal subject is common to the scholarly treatment of profanity, with authors dedicating whole chapters (see, e.g., McWhorter, 2021, Chapter 2), or using it as the main exemplar for a broader discussion of the general topic of profanity (Adams, 2016). Somewhat perversely, people have studied the word, even when using the actual word is so terrible that the author opts to only obliquely infer its existence (Read, 1934). Focusing on fuck here has a useful methodological quality as well, as its primacy of place allows theorizing guided by the most extreme example of a phenomenon. Lesser profanities—asshole, bitch, dick—among others, are prevalent in daily police verbiage (Van Maanen, 1978) and can readily fall under the umbrella of a police policy that addresses the worst of words.

Police and Profanity

If to be human is to curse, then any single study of police cursing will always fall short. There is no part of the human brain labelled “police cursing.”2 However, the aim here is not to provide a complete description of swearing by the police, or even of the word fuck. Rather, the goal is to provide some reasonable, practitioner-useable approach to regulating police profanity. The language habits of police officers have long been of interest to scholars. Some studies even include police verbal commands: for example, within measures of police use of force (Queirós et al., 2013). However, there is no empirical evidence for understanding profanity as true harm (Jay, 2009), particularly given the ubiquity of profanity in modern life.

Police profanity has been a frequent focus of scholarship (Martaindale et al., 2023; Patton et al., 2017; Sharps et al., 2019). While not about how profanity is used by police, recent commentary suggests that the core police duty is to unfuck situations (Huey & Johnston, 2023). However, the primary literature on police profanity has been focused on the public perceptions of profanity; specifically, it has focused on how profanity shapes public dis(approval) of police actions, rather than offering a close examination of how police swear. For example, researchers have experimented with videos of police use-of-force, varying only whether the captions of the officers words contained extreme profanity or none at all (Martaindale et al., 2023). This research finds that respondents ranked the profanity-laden incident as more unreasonable than the exact same incident where the officer used no profanity. Further, when viewing two different incidents with varying levels of force and situational factors, wherein neither incident involved profanity, respondents were able to rank reasonableness between the incidents. However, when profanity was introduced, both incidents were ranked at about the same level of unreasonableness. This finding suggests that profanity is capable of undermining the ability of the public to make nuanced determinations about police use of force. This study also demonstrates the ubiquity of profanity, almost to the point of saturation, as can be seen the following transcripts:(emphasis mine, Martaindale et al., 2022, app. A):

[Domestic Violence Scenario] (0:18) Officer: This stupid motherfucker is coming home. (0:24) ***Informs dispatch suspect returned*** (0:48) Officer: Stop moving, motherfucker. (0:50) Officer: I said stop fucking moving right now. (0:52) Officer: Get on the goddamn ground right now, motherfucker. (0:56) Officer: I’m not going to tell you again, dumbass. Get on the fucking ground. (1:00) Officer: Stop fucking moving and get your ass on the ground. (1: 04) Officer: Don’t you fucking reach towards that truck, motherfucker. (1:08) Officer: You stupid motherfucker, I told you to get on the goddamn ground. (1:12) Officer: But your ignorant ass didn’t listen to my commands. (1:17) Officer: Fucking idiot...give me your hand. (1:20) Officer: I said give me your fucking hand. (1:25) Officer: Stop resisting. Fuck, your dumbass doesn’t listen.

[Traffic Stop Scenario] (0:19) Officer: I need you to get out of this fucking car, right now. (0:23) Officer: You have an open arrest warrant. (0:28) Officer: So, get the fuck out of this car. You can sort it out in the jail. (0:32) Officer: I said get the fuck out of the car, or I’m going to have to drag you out of the car. (0:40) Officer: You need to fucking listen to me. Get the fuck out of this fucking car, right now. (0:46) Officer: Fuck it, I’m taking your dumbass out of this car. (0:51) Officer: Stop fucking resisting, motherfucker. (0:55) Officer: Stop resisting and get the fuck out of this goddamn car. (0:59) Officer: Stop fucking resisting. (1:04) Officer: Get on the fucking ground, motherfucker. (1:09) Officer: Stop fighting me, motherfucker. Put your hands behind your damn back. (1:15) Officer: Stop resisting, motherfucker. You’re under arrest. (1:20) Officer: I said stop fucking resisting. Fuck, just listen to me and put your hands behind your goddamn back, motherfucker.

In the domestic violence scenario, the hypothetical officer uses fuck (or a derivative) twelve times in just over one minute, and scatters in a pinch of another six profanities for good measure. During the traffic stop, just one-minute of interaction produces seventeen fucks and another dash of “dumbasses” and “goddamns.” The experimental model generates high internal validity, but to generalize to the external environment, we first should understand how well this level of profanity-in-use matches real world cases. Further, we might imagine that “goddamn car” or “stop fucking moving” are lesser moral violations (and thus less likely to sway sensitive public ears) compared to demeaning and obviously derogatory uses like “stupid motherfucker” and calling a driver “dumbass.”

In other words, scholars know quite a bit about how extreme police profanity is interpreted, but gloss over how police use profanity, thereby stalling efforts to smartly constrain its use. Capturing officers own words is a valuable effort on its own (Paoline & Terrill, 2011), but also has important implications for scholarly understanding (Mourtgos & Adams, 2019). Perhaps the most famous effort was not about profanity per se, but about how police categorize the individuals they contact (Van Maanen, 1978), which opens with [p. 307], “I guess what our job really boils down to is not letting the assholes take over the city.” Van Maanen’s brilliance was in capturing how police talk, and from that deriving three “ideal types” of citizens: suspicious persons, know-nothings, and assholes. While none of the ideal types were complimentary, suffice it to say the assholes were in the worst spot when police came calling. And while profanity was not the scholarly focus in Van Mannen’s writing, readers could not help but attend to the many profane statements that were captured. In addition to the titular assholes, cops were captured talking about “those goddamn kids,” “shitheads” and “little bastards,” any of whom may be subject to a “kick in the ass.”

Policy for the Profane Police

Concern about how the police use profanity has a rather lengthy history, as do attempts to regulate this form of police behavior. The most basic goal of police policy is to provide control and guidance to officers’ discretion, and whenever policy is too broad, it fails at inception (Alpert & Smith, 1994; Noble & Alpert, 2008). In agencies where profanity is regulated by policy, it typically falls under the “unprofessional conduct” category of discipline, which is subject to wide variation with regard to the seriousness of applicable consequences (Noble & Alpert, 2008). These unpredictable disciplinary systems are often seen as unfair by officers who fall under their ambit, and can “lead to outcomes that are detrimental for the officers, their agencies, and the communities they serve” (Worden et al., 2023, p. 22). Basic fairness in how policy is created and used is therefore a critical component of maintaining the organizational justice climate in an agency (Wolfe & Lawson, 2020).

In my review of dozens of major police agency policies, the most common policy response to profanity is to ignore it, or leave the policy broad and therefore open to interpretation. For example, the Oklahoma City Police Department (2023) mentions profanity five times: 1) to prohibit it during radio transmissions, 2) to avoid it during interviews of drivers and witnesses during vehicle collision investigations, 3) to remind officers it is a violation of FCC provisions to use profane language, 4) to avoid “harsh, violent, profane, and insolent language” as part of public courtesy, and 5) to proscribe “profane or explicit” language in email and internet content. In the Palo Alto Police Department (Palo Alto Police Department, 2022, r. 340.5.5(g)), there is a broad-reaching, qualified prohibition:

“No member shall use profane language while on-duty or in uniform, except under limited circumstances wherein the profane language is used as a deliberate verbal tactic with the specific intent to gain compliance or avoid a physical confrontation with an individual who is non-compliant, hostile or aggressive. Once such an individual complies and/or no longer poses a threat, officers are expected to refrain from continuing to use profane language.

Blanket bans on profanity are quite common in US policing, and they reflect common scholarly advice on the subject. In Michigan, police recruits are instructed that that the ethical demands of being a police officer require that they (Michigan Commission on Law Enforcement Standards, 2022, p. 3), “use proper language, [and that] profanity is always unprofessional.” Martaindale et al. (2023, p. 204) agree. They contend that profanity can negatively influence public perceptions and make the following recommendation based on the results of their study (above): “Police agencies should prohibit the use of profanity by their officers.” However, this is too much of a leap. The fact that profanity influences perceptions of the police in rare use-of-force incidents (Alpert & Dunham, 2004; McLean et al., 2022) does not justify a blanket prohibition of profanity for all police activity, given how broad those activities are (Lum et al., 2022; Ratcliffe, 2021). Rather it should be an invitation to examine how that profanity is used with the understanding that the texture of language—that is the situational and interpersonal context of its use—matters a great deal, and that a one-size-fits-all response would be viewed as both inequitable and impractical.

Police Profanity in Use

Is it unlikely that any single written work can safely capture all forms of profanity in use, or even the varied uses of a single ur-profanity within a single profession. Still, the map is made more legible by restricting our view to a single, but profound, version of profanity. As discussed earlier, “fuck” is the uber English curse word, “the word that has the deepest stigma of any in the language” (Read, 1934, p. 264), and one that “most people would consider a profanity in any situation” (Adams, 2016, p. 25). In some ways, the core assumption of this study is that fuck matters, a lot. If the reader does not bear that assumption, the rest does not follow.

Humans swear at work (Baruch et al., 2017), and cops are humans. Insofar as “swearing is not necessarily impolite, inasmuch as offensive language is often used within the boundaries of what is considered situationally appropriate in discourse” (Jay & Janschewitz, 2008, p. 268), policy that ignores those contextual boundaries is likely to fail. What of, for example, a scenario where an officer simply exclaims, “oh fuck!” upon confronting a suddenly existential threat? The Palo Alto policy example above, taken at its word, would deem such a use as against policy because it was not a “deliberate verbal tactic.” This is not mere word play – officers do face such sudden threats, they do respond to them with non-deliberate profanity, and policy does prohibit such an act. But few would disagree that the context of the situation does not violate the spirit of the policy, which makes a good faith attempt to take situational characteristics into account. The appropriate aim for police policy, it suggests, is situationally inappropriate profanity. The Palo Alto example above tacitly agrees with this aim, and attempts to cabin off a specific use-case. However, it still fails to realistically and humanely confront profanity in the police workplace.

The Police Executive’s Position

In conversations with police executives about profanity policy, those who had no policy were unanimous in describing why: “There’s no way to enforce this, my guys swear all the time, and if I jam one up, they’ll beat the write-up because we’re not consistent,” said one deputy chief at a large southeastern municipal police department. This complaint is heartfelt but ultimately unsatisfactory, choosing to avoid regulating any behavior in favor of treating recognizably different behavior as homogenous. Other executives either explicitly or implicitly recognized that there were degrees of profanity, though not all were able to capably describe those differences using policy language.

One pushback from a few police executives was they felt that using general policy cutouts for “professional behavior” provided enough of a backstop that they could use it to regulate undesirable profanity. Such a policy cutout is common in policing, but is uncommonly used for profanity violations, idiosyncratic in use, and unsuccessful when defended against in legal proceedings. When pressed, no police executive could provide a sample case where profanity was the singular reason such a policy was used to discipline an officer. In several cases, it was an addendum to other, more serious administrative sanctions, but never on its own. This is akin to having no position on profanity at all, but instead is merely an enhancement of administrative charges at the whim of the chief executive.

A View from the Line Officers

To better grasp how and when the word fuck is used, I undertook a personal effort to recall how I had used the word or heard it used by fellow officers during my 12-year career as an officer. Those years meant that I did not require academic studies to know police officers are, like people in any other profession, profane at work (Baruch et al., 2017). I then spoke with officers I knew professionally or through academic connections, which was admittedly a sample of convenience. I communicated with a total of twenty-eight current or former officers over the course of six weeks, at which point I felt the subject had reached its saturation point, as no new derivates of fuck were forthcoming. In no case did I strike any reported use of fuck – the list below is complete. However, I did conflate different conjugations of the same word (e.g., “buddy fuck” versus “buddy fucked” versus “buddy fucking”, all of which appeared), into a single example (“buddy fucker”).

As my convenience sample generated varying uses of the word fuck, I asked the contributor to tell a story where the word had been used. In all cases, the description was sufficient to allow me to generate a general definition of the word. The beginning of these conversations was unstructured and friendly. Eventually, I would shift the focus of the conversation to profanity by asking a variation of: “Can you tell me about different ways you’ve heard the word ‘fuck’ used by you or other officers during your career?” Next, I attempted to prompt them with a use of the word they had not mentioned. For example, when a conversation had stalled on their own provisions of the word fuck, I would tell them about a humorous version I already had listed (“Fucksultant” and “Fuckenomics” proved useful several times). Typically, this ploy was successful in generating several more examples from my interviewees, as they recalled other unique derivations. In no case did I present the entire listing of fuck derivations to any interviewee, as this felt too likely to generate “unique” uses that were more the product of competitive cops trying to out-do their peers.

These were casual communications, and the time they served as offficers covered a broad period of time, going as far back as 1969 and up to the present day. The group was primarily male (24 male/4 female), and roughly evenly distributed between US regions (4 West Coast, 5 Mountain West, 5 Southwest, 3 Midwest, 4 Northeast, 7 Southeast). Seventeen of the officers had spent most of their careers in agencies employing 100 or more officers, potentially biasing the sampled language towards the largest agencies in the US. In the end, fifty unique cases of fuck derivatives used by police officers were captured, and these are presented alongside a definition in Table 1.

Table 1: “Fuck” Uses by Police

Buddy fucker

An officer who intentionally makes another officer’s job more difficult, usually out of malice or self-interest.

Clusterfuck

A chaotic, disorganized, or complicated situation that is difficult to manage or navigate.

Flying Fuck

An expression of extreme indifference or disregard for a situation or person.

Fubar

An acronym for “Fucked Up Beyond All Recognition,” describing a situation that is severely damaged or beyond repair.

Fuck-all

A slang term for “nothing” or “very little,” used to express a lack of importance or value.

Fuck-around-itis

A lack of focus, discipline, or commitment in one's work, leading to inefficiency or poor performance.

Fuckathon

An extended period or series of frustrating, challenging, or overwhelming events.

Fuckbrained

A term describing a person who consistently makes poor decisions or exhibits irrational behavior.

Fucked

A situation or problem that has gone awry or become problematic.

Fucked over

To be treated unfairly or to experience a significant disadvantage due to someone else's actions.

Fuckenomics

A sarcastic term used to describe a flawed or poorly thought-out economic policy or financial decision, particularly used in response to pay and/or benefits.

Fuckeration

A term for the collective group of people who exhibit undesirable or negative qualities.

Fuckery

Deceitful or underhanded behavior, typically involving manipulation or dishonesty.

Fuckery-footed

A term for someone who is clumsy or awkward, often causing problems or disruptions.

Fuckfest

A situation marked by chaos, confusion, or disarray, often to a comical or exaggerated degree.

Fucking A

An expression of enthusiastic agreement or strong approval.

Fucking Oh

An expression of extreme surprise or disbelief.

Fuckload

A colloquial term for a large amount or quantity, often used for emphasis.

Fuckmonger

A person who instigates conflict, trouble, or negative situations.

Fucknado

A whirlwind of chaos, confusion, or destructive behavior, often attributed to a specific person or event.

Fucknut

A term used to describe a person who behaves foolishly or is perceived as lacking competence.

Fuckometer

A hypothetical measure of the level of chaos, difficulty, or frustration in a situation.

Fuckparade

A series of unfortunate, frustrating, or disappointing events.

Fuckproof

A person or situation that is resistant to failure or impervious to negative outcomes.

Fuckquake

A significant and disruptive event or situation that causes widespread chaos or upheaval.

Fucksational

An ironic term used to describe a situation or event as being particularly unpleasant or undesirable.

Fucksicle

A term used to describe someone who is both irritating and cold-hearted.

Fuckslinger

A person who frequently uses profanity or vulgar language, especially in a confrontational manner.

Fucksmith

A person who is exceptionally skilled at creating or navigating difficult or chaotic situations.

Fucksplosion

A sudden and intense outburst of anger, chaos, or confusion.

Fuckstick

A derogatory term for a person who is considered incompetent, foolish, or annoying.

Fuckstorm

A chaotic and turbulent situation or person marked by high levels of stress, conflict, or confusion.

Fuckstrated

A blend of "fuck" and "frustrated," used to express intense annoyance or irritation.

Fucksultant

A sarcastic term for someone who provides unsolicited or unhelpful advice in difficult situations, or injects difficulty into a previously controlled situation.

Fucktacular

A sarcastic term describing a situation or event as being remarkably bad or disastrous.

Fucktard

A derogatory term for someone who exhibits a lack of intelligence or common sense.

Fucktopia

A sarcastic term for an imaginary place where everything is chaotic, dysfunctional, or generally negative.

Fucktose intolerant

A term for someone who has a low tolerance for incompetence, foolishness, or irritating behavior.

Fuck-up

A mistake, error, or failure, often attributed to an individual's incompetence or negligence.

Fuckupitude

The quality or state of being prone to making mistakes or causing problems.

Fuckventory

A list or collection of mistakes, failures, or negative experiences, often used to emphasize the scale or impact of a problematic situation.

Fuckweasel

A derogatory term for a person who is deceitful, manipulative, or underhanded in their actions.

Fuckwit

A derogatory term for a person who is considered foolish, ignorant, or lacking common sense.

Motherfucker

A term used to describe a person in a derogatory manner, often due to their actions or attitude.

Ratfucker

An officer who cooperates with internal affairs against a fellow officer, often seen as a betrayal.

Snafu

An acroncym (“Situation Normal All Fucked Up”) a situation where things have gone awry or become problematic, but it is considered a typical or expected occurrence.

Unfuckable

A situation that is so challenging or complex that it seems impossible to resolve or improve. Alternatively, an especially unattractive person.

Unfucked

The resolution or improvement of a previously problematic situation.

Unfuckwithable

A person who is highly skilled, competent, or untouchable in their area of expertise.

One immediate lesson stood out after my survey of how police use fuck in the workplace. Much like the findings of studies of profanity in other workplace settings, police use fuck and its variations in a large variety of ways. Even words with seemingly straightforward negative connotations, like buddyfucker, were readily placed into positive use by my informants. For example, when asked to give an example of its use, one former officer offered, “It was my turn to buy drink at the bar, but my sergeant buddyfucked me and closed the tab before I could grab it.” The language-in-use among police is the key limiting factor to describing what useful policy in this area might look like. After all, if every “motherfucker” could be good, bad, or indifferent, how can we hope to regulate it?

A Profanity Framework

Executives and line officers both agreed there is a line of propriety, though they differed in where that line was, and how to define it. One consistent demarcation was that it was almost always permissible to swear in the presence of colleagues, and most interviewees agreed swearing at colleagues was in bounds so long as it was positive or humorous. All interviewees agreed that swearing to oneself, even when recorded, was not worth disciplining. On the other hand, swearing during public interactions, especially in a derogatory way, was generally considered less professional, although with looser boundaries and more disagreement for profanity occurring during use-of-force situations.

This is a particularly delicate area of policy, given the experimental findings that the public is less approving of an officer’s use-of-force when profanity is used (Martaindale et al., 2023; Sharps et al., 2019). Manifestly, agencies should be profoundly interested in maintaining public goodwill and trust (Tyler, 2004). However, they should also be interested in protecting officers from unnecessary internal investigation and avoiding any policy that is disconnected from the realities of policing such that officer morale suffers (Nix & Wolfe, 2016; Wolfe & Lawson, 2020). A consistent feature of police stress research is that organizational burdens drive stress levels more than features of public-facing police work (Shane, 2010, 2019), and that overly burdensome policy is a recognized driver of police stress (Worden et al., 2023). High levels of perceived organizational justice also protect against negative policing outcomes such as misconduct (Wolfe & Piquero, 2011).

Further, profanity exists in all professional walks of life, and is deemed beneficial to the people in those professions (Baruch et al., 2017). Upwards of 7% of all Twitter posts contain profanity (Laboreiro & Oliveira, 2014). Doctors swear (Perrault et al., 2022), people swear when they text (Mak & Lee, 2015), and men and women both swear (Selnow, 1985). Whe even the Pope says fuck (Chappell, 2014), it would be very odd to expect police officers to possess the saintliest mouths among us.

In other words, this is an area that requires carefully balanced policy. If the policy is too broad, then we end up perpetuating the situation we have today, where agencies either have no policy, or an unenforceable one. If the policy is too narrow, then the resulting policy is unlikely to be applied in disciplinary hearings, simply because the policy framers did not anticipate the exact formation of the profanity in question and administrative action is likely to be inconsistently applied (and therefore easily defended by the perpetrator).

Navigating the tightrope of policy creation in this arena is a daunting task, as useful policy must not only reflect societal norms, but also respect the dynamic, demanding nature of law enforcement work. To effectively frame a policy that is both realistic and enforceable, my conversations with line and executive officers lead to an approach that categorizes profanity based on its inherent characteristics. This framework, rather than approaching the issue with a broad brush, considers four pivotal aspects: intensity, form, intent, and target.

  • Intensity: Categorize the terms based on the intensity of the emotions or situations they describe, such as mild, moderate, or severe. This would help in understanding the degree of impact these words may have on the listener or reader.

  • Form: Group the terms according to their linguistic structure, such as compound words (e.g., "fucknut"), blends (e.g., "fuckstrated"), acronyms (e.g., "FUBAR"), or standalone words (e.g., "motherfucker"). This categorization would provide insights into the creative and flexible use of language.

  • Intent: Classify the terms based on the intent behind their usage, such as derogatory (e.g., intended to insult, demean, or belittle), positive (e.g., expressing admiration, support, or camaraderie), or neutral (e.g., neither insulting nor supportive, but rather used for emphasis or as a filler). This categorization would provide insights into the emotional context in which profanity is used and how it may influence the perception of the speaker.

  • Target: Categorize the terms according to their target, such as self-directed (e.g., describing one's own emotions or actions), other-directed (e.g., directed at another person or group), or situation-directed (e.g., referring to a particular event or circumstance). This would help in understanding how these terms are used to convey different perspectives or assign responsibility.

Each category presents unique facets of profanity, providing a nuanced understanding of its use. However, the utility of these categories varies when translating them into practical, enforceable policies. The first two categories – intensity and form – while interesting academically, prove challenging to implement on an operational level due to their subjectivity and complexity, while the last two categories – intent and target – offer more pragmatic and effective tools for policy creation.

When we examine intensity and form it quickly becomes clear that they vary a great deal and are subjective and mired in minutiae, such that there can be no consistent way of using them as a means of assessment. For example, intensity relies on the perceived emotional responses of the other as the decision point. Moreover, whether or not a suspect is personally offended, and to what degree, by being called “fuckbrained” is somewhat extraneous to a policy which is designed to regulate professionalism. Form, too, is less than helpful from a policy perspective. I have avoided providing every conjugation of a single fuck derivative (i.e., “buddy fuck” and “buddy fucker” and “buddy fucking”), and any policy that tried to parse these as different would immediately confront the same problem that currently exists in this arena. Specifically, that project would immediately be overwhelming and impractical with the result that there would either be no policy at all, or that the policy would be so ungainly as to be both non-navigable and unenforceable because of its amorphous nature. Nonetheless, attempts to regulate profanity according to how the language is received, or in light of its “intensity,” are commonly found in policy recommendations. Even well-regarded training and policy organizations, such as the California Commission on Police Officers Standards and Training (POST), have struggled to produce effective profanity recommendations. For example, their focus on considering the “volume, location, and proximity to others” of profanity, but not its target or intent, falls short of a clear-eyed solution to this ongoing problem (Commission, 2020, p. 53).

However, intent and target provide a foundation that make policy comprehensible to officers, and useable for their managers. By focusing on these two aspects, agencies can develop guidelines that are easier to enforce and more closely aligned with the goals of maintaining professionalism and public trust. A policy that differentiates between the use of profanity based on its intent, such as derogatory, positive, or neutral language would be both clear, contextually oriented, and enforceable. By offering clear definitions and examples, officers would have a better understanding of what is deemed professional and what is not. Additionally, agencies could take into account the target of the profanity, such as whether it is self-directed, other-directed, or situation-directed.

By concentrating on the intent and the target of profane language, policy-makers (and experimental researchers) could craft guidelines that are not only more enforceable, but also more adaptable to the varied situations officers encounter in their line of work. This approach would enable agencies to provide clearer guidance without becoming overly restrictive or disconnected from the realities of policing.

Categorization

Scholars and practitioners would benefit from a systematic way to categorize the different uses of the word "fuck" in the context of law enforcement. Early scholarship subjected different profane (or adjacent) words to factor analysis, but the resulting structure is only useful in describing that profanities load across various factors, including “euphemistic-colloquial” and “abrasive” and “abrasive-expletive” (Mabry, 1975).

Based on the language-in-use discussion above, I suggest a straightforward categorization. Rather than attempting to regulate certain words, or forms of words, a policy-oriented categorization proceeds according to whether they primarily target self/situation, colleagues, or the public, and according to the function they serve in communication—whether they are used derogatorily, positively, or neutrally.

Target Definitions:

  • The Self or Situation Directed category is when profanity was primarily referencing the officer themselves, or when describing a particular event, circumstance, or state.

  • The Colleague-directed category is when profanity was primarily directed at a colleague.

  • The Public-directed category is when profanity was primarily directed at a member of the public.

Intent definitions:

  • The Derogatory category encompasses terms used to belittle, offend, or express disapproval. This category of profanity is characterized by its intent to insult, criticize, or demean an individual or a group. These terms are often employed to assert dominance, express frustration, or convey negative sentiments, reflecting the complex emotional landscape of law enforcement interactions.

  • The Positive category contains language used to express solidarity, encouragement, or positive affirmation. Such profanity intends to foster rapport, demonstrate approval, or cultivate an atmosphere of mutual understanding. Central to this category is the intent behind the use of these terms: to communicate positive sentiments or support, regardless of the recipient..

  • The Neutral category includes terms that articulate emotions, attitudes, or feelings, typically utilized to underscore or augment a statement, or to offer a dispassionate or informative depiction of a person, situation, or state. This category is marked by its use of profanity in a manner that is neither overtly positive nor derogatory, often serving as a linguistic tool to enhance communication or express sincerity.

While the categorization is not exhaustive and may not cover all possible permutations, it provides a starting point for understanding the various ways in which the word "fuck" is used by law enforcement officers. Theory does not need to be maximally simple, but demands for ever-more nuanced theory risks losing any useable theory at all (Healy, 2017). Below, I present a 3 x 3 notional table of an experimental policy framework for regulating profanity.

Table 2: Notional Table of Profanity

Target

Derogatory Intent

Positive Intent

Neutral Intent

Self or Situation Directed

Insulting oneself; Criticizing a situation

Self-motivation; Appreciating a situation

Casual remark; Describing a situation; emphasis

Colleague Directed

Insulting a colleague

Praising a colleague

Discussing a work-related matter; emphasis

Public Directed

Insulting a member of the public

Praising a member of the public

Discussing a situation with a member of the public; emphasis

Design

A theory is only as useful as its connection to the world it describes. To test the theory of police profanity proposed above, and its connection to police practice, I constructed a pre-registered experimental test of the theory.3 The experiment systematically varied the target (self or situation directed, colleague directed, and public directed) and the context (derogatory, positive, neutral) of the profanity-in-use.

This study uses original data, collected from an online survey experiment administered to a large sample of local government (municipal and county) executives, both appointed and elected. The underlying data is drawn from a novel panel set of all municipal executives between 2013-2018, taken at six-month intervals. This data was developed by the author and a colleague. Every observation in the data is at the city role level and tracks ten municipal executive roles. For the purposes of this study, the roles of interest are public executives of human resources and law enforcement. These are two roles in local government with prominent responsibility for developing police personnel policy. In addition to role, the panel data includes information about the locality type, budgetary information, employee counts, and population counts for the locality.

Based on these criterion, 22,468 observations were initially screened for inclusion, forming the sampling frame for this study. However, upon distribution through Qualtrics, 40 emails failed, 4,423 emails bounced, and 75 records had duplicate emails, leaving a final distribution sample of 18,100. The study was held open for eleven days, beginning on July 10, 2023. Two reminder emails (four and seven days after) were sent during the open study period. A total of 2,053 respondents began (and 1,444 finished) the survey, giving a response rate of 11.3%. This rate is just slightly lower than the average response for survey experiments on political elites (Kertzer & Renshon, 2022), but substantially higher than most police-based survey research using similar online methodology (Nix et al., 2019). It is on a par with the most recent similar effort, which was a survey experiment on police chiefs and sheriffs (Adams et al., 2022).

Hypotheses

This study used a mixed design with both within-subjects and between-subjects conditions. Participants were randomly exposed to four out of nine possible conditions, with each condition corresponding to a different vignette involving the use of the profanity "fuck" by a police officer. These conditions are designed based on a 3 (target: self or situation-directed, colleague-directed, public-directed) x 3 (intent: derogatory, positive, neutral) matrix, yielding a total of nine conditions.

Respondents were exposed to four randomly selected conditions of the nine available. Following each vignette, respondents rated the appropriateness, professionalism, impact to public trust, policy-dictated discipline, and personal preference for discipline for the scenario they had just seen. The vignettes followed a prompt that provided consistent background information on each scenario presented to respondents:

“Imagine the following has occurred in your agency. The situation came to your attention through a supervisor who was randomly reviewing body-worn camera footage. There has been no complaint at this time, but the situation remains under investigation.”

Following that prompt, one of the four randomly selected vignettes appeared. Following each vignette, respondents were asked to respond to five outcome questions. Below, each outcome question is linked to formal hypotheses related to the two experimental conditions of Target and Intent. All hypotheses were pre-registered, and the appendix to this article includes the original preregistration document.

Perceived Acceptability: "In your opinion, how acceptable was this officer’s use of profanity at work?" Participants responded on a Likert scale from 1 (completely unacceptable) to 5 (completely acceptable).

  • H1A: The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as more acceptable than when other-directed (colleague or public).

  • H2A: Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as more acceptable than derogatory intent.

Perceived Professionalism: "In your opinion, how professional was this officer in using profanity at work?" Participants responded on a Likert scale from 1 (completely unprofessional) to 5 (completely professional).

  • H1B: The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as more professional than when other-directed (colleague or public).

  • H2B: Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as more professional than derogatory intent.

Impact on Public Trust: "If made public, how do you think this officer’s use of profanity would affect public trust in the police?" Participants responded on a Likert scale from 1 (greatly reduces public trust) to 5 (greatly enhances public trust).

  • H1C: The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as having less negative impact on public trust than when other-directed (colleague or public).

  • H2C: Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as having less negative impact on public trust than derogatory intent.

Disciplinary Action (Agency Policy): "How severe would the disciplinary action be, according to your agency's policy, for this officer?" Participants responded on a Likert scale from (1) no sanction, (2) verbal coaching, (3) written warning, (4) significant sanction such as time off, and (5) termination of employment.

  • H1D: The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as warranting less severe policy-based disciplinary action than when other-directed (colleague or public).

  • H2D: Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as warranting less severe policy-based disciplinary action than derogatory intent.

Disciplinary Action (Personal): "In your personal opinion, how severe should the disciplinary action be (without regard to your agency policy) for this officer?" Participants responded on a Likert scale from (1) no sanction, (2) verbal coaching, (3) written warning, (4) significant sanction such as time off, and (5) termination of employment.

  • H1E: The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as warranting less severe personal opinion-based disciplinary action than when other-directed (colleague or public).

  • H2E: Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as warranting less severe personal opinion-based disciplinary action than derogatory intent.

Sample

The sample for this study consisted of 1,492 participants who were predominantly serving in leadership roles within their respective organizations. Responses were measured across multiple assessments per respondent, generating an observations’ set of 5,280 responses. Most participants (72.3%) were heads of law enforcement, while 24.7% were heads of human resources. A small proportion (3.0%) did not specify their role. Respondents come from all 50 US states.

The participants were experienced professionals, with an average of 27.5 years (SD = 10.0) of professional experience and an average of 10.7 years (SD = 10.2) of experience in their current role. The average age of the participants was 53.7 years (SD = 8.8). The sample was predominantly white (82.9%), with small representations of individuals identifying as Black or African American (3.8%), more than one race (3.2%), Hispanic (2.7%), American Indian or Alaska Native (0.5%), Asian (0.2%), and Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander (0.1%). 6.6% did not specify their race. The participants were highly educated, with 31.5% holding a master’s degree, 29.1% holding a bachelor’s degree, 16.7% holding an associate’s degree, and 13.4% having completed high school or equivalent. A small proportion (3.5%) held doctoral-level qualifications such as PhD, JD, or MD. Most participants were male (76.9%), with females comprising 16.5% of the sample.

The organizations represented in the sample were predominantly municipalities (50.8%), with a smaller proportion of county (11.1%) and township (9.5%) governments. 28.6% did not specify their governmental category. The average city population served by these organizations was 38,212.8 (SD = 138,467.2), and they had an average of 42.3 full-time officers (SD = 121.1). The mean expenditure on police services was $6,749,400 (SD = $24,065,400).

Appendix Table A1 reports treatment balance across covariates, and in all cases the randomization procedure was effective.

Table 3: Sample Descriptive Statistics, n=1,492

Mean

Std. Dev.

Current Role Experience

10.7

10.2

Professional Experience

27.5

10.0

Age

53.7

8.8

City Population

38212.8

138467.2

Police Expenditures

6749.4

24065.4

Fulltime Officers

42.3

121.1

N

Pct.

Role

Head of HR

334

24.7

Head of Law Enforcement

979

72.3

NA

41

3.0

Gov. Category

county

150

11.1

municipality

688

50.8

township

129

9.5

NA

387

28.6

Education

HS or equivalent

182

13.4

Associate's

226

16.7

Bachelor's

394

29.1

Master's

426

31.5

PhD, JD, MD, etc.

47

3.5

Race

American Indian or Alaska Native

7

0.5

Asian

3

0.2

Black or African American

51

3.8

Hispanic

37

2.7

More than one race

43

3.2

Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander

1

0.1

White

1123

82.9

NA

89

6.6

Sex

Male

1041

76.9

Female

16.5

Note: Police expenditures reported in $1000 increments

Analytic Plan

The primary independent variables are the experimental conditions target (self, colleague, public) and intent (neutral, positive, derogatory), resulting in a 3x3 main effects experiment. I employed a mixed effects model to analyze the data, which was the result of each respondent answering a random selection of four vignettes drawn from the nine possible. Analysis proceeded with the `lmer` package (Bates et al., 2015) in R (R Core Team, 2023). The general form of the mixed effects model is represented by the following multilevel equation:

OutcomeiN(μ,σ2)μ=αj[i]+β1(TargetColleague)+β2(TargetPublic) +β3(IntentDerogatory)+β4(IntentPositive)αjN(μαj,σαj2), for ResponseId j = 1,,J\begin{matrix} {Outcome}_{i} & \sim N\left( \mu,\sigma^{2} \right) \\ \mu & = \alpha_{j\lbrack i\rbrack} + \beta_{1}\left( {Target}_{Colleague} \right) + \beta_{2}\left( {Target}_{Public} \right)\ + \\ & \quad\beta_{3}\left( {Intent}_{Derogatory} \right) + \beta_{4}\left( {Intent}_{Positive} \right) \\ \alpha_{j} & \sim N\left( \mu_{\alpha_{j}},\sigma_{\alpha_{j}}^{2} \right)\text{,\ for\ ResponseId\ j\ =\ 1,}\ldots\text{,J} \\ \end{matrix}

The dependent variable, Outcome{i}Outcome\{ i\} for the iith observation, is assumed to follow a normal distribution with mean μ\mu and variance σ2\sigma^{2}. The mean μ\mu is modeled as a linear combination of several predictor variables. The αj[i]\alpha_{j\lbrack i\rbrack} is a random intercept for each participant jj associated with observation ii, while β1\beta_{1} through β4\beta_{4} represent the fixed effects coefficients for each predictor.4 The predictors TargetColleague{Target}_{Colleague}, TargetPublic{Target}_{Public}, IntentDerogatory{Intent}_{Derogatory}, and IntentPositive {Intent}_{Positive}\ are binary variables indicating the target and intent of the profanity use. These are the fixed effects in the mixed effects model.

The random intercept αj\alpha_{j} for each participant jj is assumed to follow a normal distribution with mean μαj\mu_{\alpha_{j}} and variance σαj2\sigma_{\alpha_{j}}^{2}. This captures the individual differences among participants that affect their ratings and are not captured by the fixed effects.

The interaction model presented later includes an interaction term between the main experimental variables of Target and Intent, resulting in a mixed effects model represented by a similar multilevel equation with more terms to represent the interaction:

OutcomeiN(μ,σ2)μ=αj[i]+β1(TargetColleague) +β2(TargetPublic)+β3(IntentDerogatory) +β4(IntentPositive)+β5(IntentDerogatory×TargetColleague) +β6(IntentDerogatory×TargetPublic)+β7(IntentPositive×TargetColleague) +β8(IntentPositive×TargetPublic)αjN(μαj,σαj2), for ResponseId j = 1,,J\begin{matrix} {Outcome}_{i} & \sim N\left( \mu,\sigma^{2} \right) \\ \mu & = \alpha_{j\lbrack i\rbrack} + \beta_{1}\left( {Target}_{Colleague} \right)\ + \\ & \quad\beta_{2}\left( {Target}_{Public} \right) + \beta_{3}\left( {Intent}_{Derogatory} \right)\ + \\ & \quad\beta_{4}\left( {Intent}_{Positive} \right) + \beta_{5}\left( {Intent}_{Derogatory} \times {Target}_{Colleague} \right)\ + \\ & \quad\beta_{6}\left( {Intent}_{Derogatory} \times {Target}_{Public} \right) + \beta_{7}\left( {Intent}_{Positive} \times {Target}_{Colleague} \right)\ + \\ & \quad\beta_{8}\left( {Intent}_{Positive} \times {Target}_{Public} \right) \\ \alpha_{j} & \sim N\left( \mu_{\alpha_{j}},\sigma_{\alpha_{j}}^{2} \right)\text{,\ for\ ResponseId\ j\ =\ 1,}\ldots\text{,J} \\ \end{matrix}

Results

Overall, results provide strong support for the study's hypotheses, with all but one of the ten registered hypotheses confirmed through experimental testing, and the remaining hypothesis partially supported. Appendix Table A5 includes results for all hypotheses in tabular form. Profanity use that is self/situation-directed or has a positive or neutral intent is generally perceived as more acceptable and professional, has less negative impact on public trust, and is deemed to warrant less severe disciplinary action. On the other hand, profanity that is directed at the public or has a derogatory intent is perceived more negatively across all outcomes. Table 4 reports the main experimental results for the entire sample.5 Appendix Table A5 reports the pre-registered hypotheses and associated finding in tabular form.

The target of the profanity had a significant influence on all outcome variables. When the profanity was directed at the public, it was seen as significantly less appropriate (b = -1.121, p < 0.001), less professional (b = -1.033, p < 0.001), and was perceived to have a greater negative impact on public trust (b = -0.730, p < 0.001) compared to when it was self-directed. Furthermore, profanity directed at the public was associated with a perception of needing more severe policy-based (b = 1.033, p < 0.001) and personal opinion-based (b = 1.085, p < 0.001) disciplinary action. Similarly, the use of profanity directed towards colleagues was also seen as less appropriate (b = -0.227, p < 0.001) and less professional (b = -0.226, p < 0.001), with a smaller but still significant negative impact on public trust (b = -0.065, p < 0.001). It, too, was associated with a perception of needing more severe policy-based (b = 0.167, p < 0.001) and personal opinion-based (b = 0.167, p < 0.001) disciplinary action, compared to profanity that was self-directed or situation-directed.

The intent of the profanity was also found to significantly influence perceptions. Profanity with a derogatory intent was perceived as less appropriate (b = -0.113, p < 0.001) and less professional (b = -0.111, p < 0.001) compared to profanity with a neutral intent. Interestingly, compared to a neutral intent, derogatory profanity did not have a significant impact on public trust (b = 0.003, p = n.s.), but it was associated with perceptions of needing more severe policy-based (b = 0.093, p < 0.001) and personal opinion-based (b = 0.086, p < 0.01) disciplinary action. On the other hand, profanity with a positive intent was perceived as more appropriate (b = 0.043, p < 0.05) and more professional (b = 0.040, p < 0.05), with a larger positive impact on public trust (b = 0.124, p < 0.001), and was associated with perceptions of needing less severe policy-based (b = -0.087, p < 0.001) and personal opinion-based (b = -0.087, p < 0.01) disciplinary action, compared to profanity with a neutral intent.

Indeed, while the fixed effects in the model explained a substantial portion of the variance in the outcomes, there was still a significant proportion of unexplained variance.6 This unexplained variance, ranging from around 42% to 67% across the different outcomes, could potentially be attributed to factors not directly measured in this study. One such factor is the broader "context" of the situation in which profanity is used, as pointed out by almost every chief and line officer consulted during this study. These contextual elements, such as the severity of the situation including threat dynamics, the relationship between the officer and the target of the profanity, and other situational dynamics, could have significant impacts on evaluations of the appropriateness, professionalism, impact on public trust, and disciplinary action associated with the use of profanity. This gap highlights the potential need for further research to incorporate and examine these contextual elements more explicitly. In the next section, I report on further model results, and consider organizational and respondent characteristics.

Table 4: Mixed Effects Model: Main Experimental Effects

Appropriate

Professional

Public Trust

Discipline (Policy)

Discipline (Personal)

[Target] Colleague

-0.227 (0.021)***

-0.226 (0.019)***

-0.065 (0.017)***

0.167

(0.017)***

0.167

(0.018)***

[Target]

Public

-1.121 (0.020)***

-1.033 (0.019)***

-0.730 (0.017)***

1.033

(0.017)***

1.085

(0.018)***

[Intent] Derogatory

-0.113 (0.021)***

-0.111 (0.019)***

0.003

(0.017)

0.093

(0.017)***

0.086

(0.018)***

[Intent]

Positive

0.043

(0.020)*

0.040

(0.019)*

0.124

(0.017)***

-0.087 (0.017)***

-0.087 (0.018)***

Intercept

2.500

(0.023)***

2.357

(0.021)***

2.504

(0.020)***

1.574

(0.020)***

1.594

(0.021)***

SD (Intercept Respondent)

0.473

0.431

0.476

0.464

0.497

SD (Observations)

0.568

0.526

0.474

0.489

Num.Obs.

5258

5270

5248

5246

5273

R2 Marg.

0.304

0.303

0.199

0.324

0.324

R2 Cond.

0.589

0.583

0.601

0.655

0.667

AIC

10779.8

9957.1

9238.2

9176.8

9636.2

BIC

10825.7

10003.1

9284.1

9222.7

9682.2

ICC

0.4

0.4

0.5

0.5

0.5

RMSE

0.51

0.48

0.42

0.42

0.44

Note: unstandardized coefficients and standard errors reported as “coeff. (SE).”

Controlled Models

The next step was to include observational variables in the equation. It’s important to note that because these are non-experimental observational characteristics, results should be considered preliminary. In Appendix Table A4, results for a controlled model are reported, and findings are consistent with the results reported above. With regard to the non-experimental controls, a notable finding was that participants in law enforcement roles tended to perceive profanity as more professional compared to those in HR executive roles (b = 0.100, p < 0.05), perhaps reflecting the concerns raised in my conversations with police executives who saw a role for profanity in police work. Furthermore, longer career tenure was associated with a greater negative impact on public trust (b = -0.005, p < 0.01) and a greater need for both policy-based (b = 0.007, p < 0.001) and personal opinion-based (b = 0.007, p < 0.01) disciplinary action. Education level was positively associated with perceived appropriateness (b = 0.049, p < 0.01) and professionalism (b = 0.044, p < 0.01), and it was negatively associated with the need for both policy-based (b = -0.043, p < 0.01) and personal opinion-based (b = -0.048, p < 0.01) disciplinary action.

The effects of race and sex were less consistent. Non-white participants showed a trend towards perceiving a greater need for policy-based (b = 0.106, p < 0.1) and personal opinion-based (b = 0.112, p < 0.1) disciplinary action compared to white participants. Female participants perceived a greater negative impact on public trust compared to male participants (b = -0.140, p < 0.01).

These findings provide further evidence of the influence of both the target and intent of profanity on its perceived implications, while also highlighting the role of various demographic and professional factors in shaping these perceptions. This underscores the complex nature of this issue and the need for a nuanced approach to addressing it in professional settings.

Results for Experimental Interaction

Importantly, real-world profanity is not stripped of either target or intent – they travel together, and any policy-based evaluation of an officer who uses profanity is highly likely to need to examine both elements in tandem and, in the language of statistics, to evaluate their interactive effects. Therefore, the interaction effects of the two experimental treatments, {Target} and {Intent}, were examined in a mixed effects model. Tabular results for the interaction model are reported in Appendix Table A4. This model revealed several noteworthy findings.

In all cases, the interaction model provides a significantly better fit to the data than the simple model for each of the outcome variables. See Appendix Table 4a for full results of the nested linear mixed models’ test. The p-values (Pr(>Chisq)) for the Chi-squared tests are all significant (p < 2.2e-16), suggesting that, for each outcome variable, the interaction model provides a significantly better fit to the data than the simple model. In other words, the interaction between "Target" and "Intent" significantly improves the model's ability to predict each of the outcome variables, over and above the individual effects of "Target" and "Intent".

The interaction relationships are visualized in Figure 1. First, the effect of target on the outcome variables was significantly moderated by the intent of the profanity. For example, derogatory intent amplified the negative effect of colleague-directed profanity on perceived appropriateness (b = -0.480, p < 0.001), professionalism (b = -0.396, p < 0.001), and public trust (b = -0.173, p < 0.001), and it increased the perceived severity for policy-based (b = 0.356, p < 0.001) and personal opinion-based (b = 0.400, p < 0.001) disciplinary action. However, for public-directed profanity, the derogatory intent did not exacerbate its negative impact but, instead, slightly tempered it in terms of perceived appropriateness (b = 0.117, p < 0.05) and professionalism (b = 0.161, p < 0.001).

Secondly, positive intent was found to moderate the effect of target on the outcomes. Specifically, it reduced the negative impact of colleague-directed profanity on perceived appropriateness (b = 0.114, p < 0.05), professionalism (b = 0.132, p < 0.01), and public trust (b = 0.182, p < 0.001), and it lessened the perceived need for policy-based disciplinary action (b = -0.083, p < 0.05). Interestingly, for public-directed profanity, positive intent had no significant effect on perceived appropriateness (b = 0.000, p = n.s.) but improved perceptions of professionalism (b = 0.093, p < 0.05) and significantly enhanced public trust (b = 0.292, p < 0.001), while reducing the perceived need for both policy-based (b = -0.228, p < 0.001) and personal opinion-based (b = -0.227, p < 0.01) disciplinary action.

The variance explained by the fixed effects in this model, including the interaction terms, increased slightly compared to the previous model. The marginal R-squared values ranged from 0.208 for public trust to 0.335 for disciplinary action, indicating that the inclusion of the interaction terms improved the explanatory power of the model. The conditional R-squared values, which include both the fixed and random effects, ranged from 0.611 for public trust to 0.680 for disciplinary action, demonstrating that the model fits the data well. Furthermore, the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) values were lower in this model compared to the previous model, providing additional evidence of improved model fit.

These findings underscore the complex interplay between the target and intent of profanity in shaping perceptions of its appropriateness, professionalism, impact on public trust, and the severity of disciplinary action it warrants. As such, these results highlight the importance of considering both the target and intent of profanity, as well as their interaction, in understanding and addressing the implications of profanity use in a professional setting.

A group of graphs with different colored lines Description automatically generated

Figure 1: Experimental Interaction Effects of {Target} x {Intent}

Discussion

The results suggest broad agreement between the police and local government human resources professionals that, in policing, individually targeted derogatory profanity towards colleagues should be the target of agency policy, as should any profanity directed at a member of the public. This is language that derides, demeans, and dehumanizes individuals – civilian and colleagues alike. The findings make clear the need for a reassessment of current policies surrounding the use of profanity in policing. The delineation of the target and intent of profanity proves critical in shaping perceptions of appropriateness, professionalism, and public trust, as well as influencing the degree of perceived need for disciplinary action.

Accordingly, policy recommendations should emphasize a greater focus on the context of profanity use. For instance, agencies should consider developing guidelines that distinguish between profanity directed at the public and profanity that is self-directed. The study's findings suggest that the former is seen as significantly less appropriate, and professional and is associated with a perceived need for more severe disciplinary action. Therefore, policies that specifically target such uses of profanity could serve to enhance the professionalism of officers and improve public trust in the police.

The intent of the profanity – whether it is used with a derogatory, neutral, or positive intent – is also a crucial factor to consider. Law enforcement agencies should consider policies that discourage the use of derogatory profanity while allowing room for profanity used with a positive or neutral intent in suitable contexts. This distinction may be challenging to implement in practice, as the intent can be somewhat subjective, and may not always be clear. However, compared to blanket bans or a lack of any policy control – which is the currently the prevailing policy regarding profanity in the police workplace – a clear focus on intent and target would provide far more guidance to officers, while also building a functional framework for police executives charged with policy enforcement. It’s also important to note that such a policy would also require discretion and good judgement in managing the use of profanity.

The complexity of the interaction effects observed here underscores the need to concomitantly consider both the target and the intent of profanity in policy-making. The professionalism of an officer cannot be decided with an acid test determining whether profanity was used. This is the equivalent of strict liability, wherein the act of itself is enough to find against the perpetrator – this is far too simple an approach to a situation that is both laden with context and which has significant potential impact, whether with regard to public trust or an officer’s professionalism and perceived appropriateness. Manifestly, the considerable interplay between the target and intent of profanity further highlights the inadequacy of broad or blanket policies in this domain. Rather, such policies require nuanced guidelines that can interpret the intent and target of the profanity together, thereby aligning the disciplinary action more accurately with the impact of the language used (Worden et al., 2023). This would assist in developing a fairer and more effective approach to managing and regulating language use within law enforcement agencies (Alpert & Smith, 1994; Noble & Alpert, 2008).

Few policing-focused studies would be complete without a strong recommendation on training. However, we should be cautious in making such recommendations considering the lack of compelling evidence about whether training works to accomplish our goals. Existing training programs already focus on communication skills that allow officers to command respect and authority, build rapport, and diffuse tension without resorting to the use of derogatory profanity (Engel et al., 2022; Todak & James, 2018). The findings here suggest such programs should also highlight the potential negative impacts of derogatory profanity on public trust and judgements about police actions, particularly during use-of-force incidents (Martaindale et al., 2023; Patton et al., 2017; Sharps et al., 2019). In any case, such training must reflect how police officers experience their work, otherwise it will fail. As Worden & McLean observe (2017, p. 41), “training must be tightly coupled with the day-to-day reality of officers’ experience, and if it is not, it will prove ineffective.”

Lastly, these recommendations should be contextualized within the broader policing environment. In this regard, the analysis highlights several demographic and professional factors that influence perceptions of regarding the use of profanity, and which should be considered when developing and implementing these policies. For instance, participants in law enforcement roles tended to perceive profanity as more professional than those in HR executive roles, indicating a potential cultural acceptance of profanity in certain contexts within the police force. Similarly, the more formal education a respondent had, the more approving they were of the use of profanity, while women tended to be more disapproving when compared to men. These divergent perceptions underscore the importance of considering the views of those directly affected by these policies, and of fostering open dialogues about the role of language and communication in policing.

In conclusion, the key policy implications of this study point towards a more nuanced approach to managing the use of profanity in policing. Such an approach should not only be informed by the findings presented here, but also by ongoing dialogue within law enforcement agencies, as well as further empirical research into the complex role that language plays in the day-to-day operations of the police force.

Limitations and Future Research

I have focused tightly on the most often criticized, but common expletive: fuck. That focus means a host of questions remain unaddressed in the study of police profanity. First, though this study assumes that sound policy can be constructed by focusing on the most severe forms of the prohibited activity, this assumption could be undermined by future research if it were to find that less common, or even less visceral forms of profanity, were not targetable using something similar to the policy I recommend earlier. In other words, to take the canonical academic form, how police officers use the word “asshole” may very well differ in use from “fuck” (and derivates).7 This may seem unlikely, but a future study comparing vulgarity across types would be a welcome contribution to the study of police profanity, and useful in sharpening policy recommendations.

A second limitation is rooted in the flexible nature of profanity itself. It is possible, and even likely, that officers will continue to adapt their language to the context of their profession and the challenges they face. These changes may include new forms of fuck. The way today’s officers use language is undoubtedly different from their predecessors, and it follows that tomorrow’s officers may well introduce new derivatives of the word. Our understanding of fuck and other profanity, therefore, must also be flexible and adapt to those changes.

A third limitation is that this study used a sample drawn only from US policing and human resources personnel, and was conducted in English. While fuck is a common English loan word to other languages, there is obvious cultural complexity when attempting to generalize from a US, English-language-based sample to international contexts. Therefore, similar experiments in other cultures, languages, countries, and customs, would dramatically improve our collective understanding of police profanity.

Finally, like Van Maanen’s (1978, p. 310) assholes, know nothings, and suspicious persons, the framework I propose here is “anything but precise and absolute.” Rather, it is intended to begin the policy conversation from a relatively conservative place, given the potentially significant impacts such changes would have on police-community relations. To the degree that a national practice has yet to emerge in relationship to regulating police profanity, the policy aims here should be the beginning of the discussion, and not the end of one.

Conclusion

I have attempted to lay out the language-in-use properties of the word "fuck" in policing, revealing its capacity to convey multiple meanings and functions, from emphasis and camaraderie to power and aggression. The complexity of even this single swear word puts the lie to policy that attempts to regulate through blanket bans of profanity. I proposed a theory of profanity policy and found confirmation through a large, pre-registered survey experiment issued to policing and local government human resources executives across the country. The resulting policy guidance therefore reflects a national consensus that acknowledges the nuanced contexts in which profanity may be used and advocates for a more precise and informed approach to its regulation. The inclusion of both the target and intent of profanity in decision-making processes related to policy and disciplinary actions indicates a shift towards a more balanced, contextual understanding of language use in policing. This not only enhances the professional conduct of law enforcement officers but also fosters increased public trust.

The convergence of the perspectives of policing and HR professionals in this study also represents a promising sign of broad consensus and potential collaboration. While differences do exist, the points of agreement offer a clear path towards fostering a shared understanding and approach to language policy in policing. By adopting this national consensus as the basis of the policy discourse on the use of profanity, we can better address the challenges of language use in policing and develop more effective policies and practices that respect the complexities of this issue.

References

Adams, I. T., McCrain, J., Schiff, D. S., Schiff, K. J., & Mourtgos, S. M. (2022). Public Pressure or Peer Influence: What Shapes Police Executives’ Views on Civilian Oversight? SocArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/mdu96

Adams, M. (2016). In Praise of Profanity. Oxford University Press.

Alpert, G. P., & Dunham, R. G. (2004). Understanding police use of force: Officers, suspects, and reciprocity. Cambridge Univ. Press.

Alpert, G. P., & Smith, W. C. (1994). Developing Police Policy: An Evaluation of the Control Principle. American Journal of Police, 13, 1.

Baruch, Y., Prouska, R., Ollier-Malaterre, A., & Bunk, J. (2017). Swearing at work: The mixed outcomes of profanity. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 32(2), 149–162. https://doi.org/10.1108/JMP-04-2016-0102

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Calvin Lai [@CalvinKLai]. (2023, February 8). @ian_t_adams To adapt a saying from psychology: “there is no special module for policing in the brain.” [Tweet]. Twitter. https://twitter.com/CalvinKLai/status/1623366487560884233

Chappell, B. (2014, March 3). Pope Francis Lets a Vulgarity Slip During Vatican Address. NPR. https://www.npr.org/sections/thetwo-way/2014/03/03/285271511/pope-francis-lets-a-vulgarity-slip-during-vatican-address

Commission, C. P. (2020). De-Escalation Strategies & Techniques for California Law Enforcement. California Commission on Police Officers Standards and Training. https://post.ca.gov/Portals/0/post_docs/publications/DeEscalation.pdf

Engel, R. S., Corsaro, N., Isaza, G. T., & McManus, H. D. (2022). Assessing the impact of de-escalation training on police behavior: Reducing police use of force in the Louisville, KY Metro Police Department. Criminology & Public Policy, 21(2), 199–233. https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9133.12574

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146

Fjeld, R. E. V., Kristiansen, E., Rathje, M., Oskarsson, V., Konstaninovskaia, N., Gill, I., & Menuta, F. (2019). The worldwide use and meaning of the f-word. Intercultural Pragmatics, 16(1), 85–111. https://doi.org/10.1515/ip-2019-0004

Frankfurt, H. G. (2009). On Bullshit. In On Bullshit. Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400826537

Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Oxford and Carlton: Blackwell Publishing.

Healy, K. (2017). Fuck Nuance. Sociological Theory, 35(2), 118–127. https://doi.org/10.1177/0735275117709046

Huey, L., & Johnston, S. (2023). Unf@cking People’s Problems: A Theory of Policing. In CrimRxiv. https://www.crimrxiv.com/pub/4l8qj9e9/release/1

Jay, T. (1999). Why we curse. Why We Curse, 1–344.

Jay, T. (2009). Do offensive words harm people? Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 15(2), 81.

Jay, T., & Janschewitz, K. (2008). The pragmatics of swearing.

Kertzer, J. D., & Renshon, J. (2022). Experiments and Surveys on Political Elites. Annual Review of Political Science, 25(1), 529–550. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-051120-013649

Laboreiro, G., & Oliveira, E. (2014). What We Can Learn from Looking at Profanity. In J. Baptista, N. Mamede, S. Candeias, I. Paraboni, T. A. S. Pardo, & M. das G. Volpe Nunes (Eds.), Computational Processing of the Portuguese Language (pp. 108–113). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-09761-9_11

Lum, C., Koper, C. S., & Wu, X. (2022). Can we really defund the police? A nine-agency study of police response to calls for service. Police Quarterly, 25(3), 255–280.

Mabry, E. A. (1975). A multivariate investigation of profane language. Central States Speech Journal, 26(1), 39–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/10510977509367817

Mak, B. C. N., & Lee, C. (2015). Swearing Is E-Business: Expletives in Instant Messaging in Hong Kong Workplaces. In E. Darics (Ed.), Digital Business Discourse (pp. 122–141). Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137405579_7

Martaindale, M. H., Sandel, W. L., Duron, A., & Blair, J. P. (2023). @#%$!: The Impact of Officer Profanity on Civilians’ Perception of What Constitutes Reasonable Use of Force. Police Quarterly, 26(2), 194–212. https://doi.org/10.1177/10986111221111332

McLean, K., Stoughton, S. W., & Alpert, G. P. (2022). Police Uses of Force in the USA: A Wealth of Theories and a Lack of Evidence. Cambridge Journal of Evidence-Based Policing, 6(3), 87–108. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41887-022-00078-7

McWhorter, J. (2021). Nine Nasty Words: English in the Gutter: Then, Now, and Forever. Penguin.

Michigan Commission on Law Enforcement Standards. (2022). Basic Training Module Specifications. Michigan Commission on Law Enforcement Standards. https://www.michigan.gov/-/media/Project/Websites/mcoles/TD-Webpage/basic_training_curriculum/ii-b-1.pdf?rev=74c97a3bee3e4a88af85bf353d6fc9fe

Mourtgos, S. M., & Adams, I. T. (2019). The rhetoric of de-policing: Evaluating open-ended survey responses from police officers with machine learning-based structural topic modeling. Journal of Criminal Justice, 64, 61–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2019.101627

Mummolo, J. (2018). Modern Police Tactics, Police-Citizen Interactions, and the Prospects for Reform. The Journal of Politics, 80(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1086/694393

Nix, J., Pickett, J. T., Baek, H., & Alpert, G. P. (2019). Police research, officer surveys, and response rates. Policing and Society, 29(5), 530–550. https://doi.org/10.1080/10439463.2017.1394300

Nix, J., & Wolfe, S. E. (2016). Sensitivity to the Ferguson Effect: The role of managerial organizational justice. Journal of Criminal Justice, 47, 12–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.06.002

Noble, J. J., & Alpert, G. P. (2008). Managing Accountability Systems for Police Conduct: Internal Affairs and External Oversight. Waveland Press.

Operations Manual 5th Edition. (2023). https://www.okc.gov/home/showpublisheddocument/34227/638162797491530000

Palo Alto Police Department. (2022). Policy Manual. https://www.cityofpaloalto.org/files/assets/public/police-department/public-information-portal/policy-manual-for-web-5-3-2023.pdf

Paoline, E. A., & Terrill, W. (2011). Listen to me! Police officers’ views of appropriate use of force. Journal of Crime and Justice, 34(3), 178–189. https://doi.org/10.1080/0735648X.2011.609740

Patton, C. L., Asken, M., Fremouw, W. J., & Bemis, R. (2017). The Influence of Police Profanity on Public Perception of Excessive Force. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 32(4), 340–357. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11896-017-9226-0

Perrault, E. K., Barton, J. A., Hildenbrand, G. M., McCullock, S. P., Lee, D. G., & Adu Gyamfi, P. (2022). When Doctors Swear, Do Patients Care? Two Experiments Examining Physicians Cursing in the Presence of Patients. Health Communication, 37(6), 739–747. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2020.1865610

Queirós, C., Kaiseler, M., & da Silva, A. L. (2013). Burnout as a predictor of aggressivity among police officers.

R Core Team. (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Manual]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/

Ratcliffe, J. H. (2021). Policing and public health calls for service in Philadelphia. Crime Science, 10(1), 5. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40163-021-00141-0

Read, A. W. (1934). An Obscenity Symbol. American Speech, 9(4), 264–278. https://doi.org/10.2307/452191

Sausdal, D. (2020). Police Bullshit: Taking Brutal Police Talk Less Seriously. Journal of Extreme Anthropology, 4(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.5617/jea.7360

Selnow, G. W. (1985). Sex differences in uses and perceptions of profanity. Sex Roles, 12(3), 303–312. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287597

Shane, J. M. (2010). Organizational stressors and police performance. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(4), 807–818. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2010.05.008

Shane, J. M. (2019). Stress Inside Police Departments. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429297229

Sharps, M. J., Torkelson, J. F., Hulett, D. L., Kuhn, M. L., & Sevillano, C. N. (2019). Police Profanity and Public Judgments of Guilt and Effectiveness in Officer-Involved Shootings. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 34(1), 87–90. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11896-018-9268-y

Todak, N., & James, L. (2018). A Systematic Social Observation Study of Police De-Escalation Tactics. Police Quarterly, 21(4), 509–543. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098611118784007

Tyler, T. R. (2004). Enhancing police legitimacy. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 593, 84–99.

Van Maanen, J. (1978). The Asshole. Policing: A View from the Street, 221–238.

Wolfe, S. E., & Lawson, S. G. (2020). The organizational justice effect among criminal justice employees: A meta-analysis*. Criminology, 58(4), 619–644. https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12251

Wolfe, S. E., & Piquero, A. R. (2011). Organizational Justice and Police Misconduct. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38(4), 332–353. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854810397739

Worden, R. E., Harris, C. J., & Kim, M. (2023). Disciplinary Sanctions for Police Misconduct: An Empirical Analysis of Sanction Severity. Police Quarterly, 10986111231174831. https://doi.org/10.1177/10986111231174831

Worden, R. E., & McLean, S. J. (2017). Mirage of Police Reform. In University of California Press. University of California Press. https://doi.org/10.1525/luminos.30

Appendix

Power Analysis

Prior to survey administration, I conducted an a priori power analysis to determine the required sample size. This analysis was based on the following parameters: an anticipated small effect size (f²) of 0.02, a significance level (α) of 0.05, and a desired statistical power of 0.80. The study design involves eight predictors in total: four dummy variables representing the tested levels of the two main predictors (intent and target), one between-subjects factor (role: police chief or human resources executive), and three control variables (age, gender, and years of experience in career). Main effects of four predictors are the main focus of the study.

The power analysis, conducted using G*Power (Faul et al., 2007), indicated that a total sample size of 602 participants would be required to achieve the desired statistical power. This is based on a noncentrality parameter λ of 12.04, a critical F-value of 2.3869601, and degrees of freedom of 4 (numerator) and 593 (denominator). The resulting actual power for this sample size is 0.8003320, meeting the target power of 0.80. Therefore, I aimed to recruit a minimum of 602 participants for this study to ensure adequate power for detecting a true small effect, if one exists, and protect against false negatives.

Full Vignette Wording

Self or Situation Directed:

  1. Derogatory Intent: Officer Smith had pulled over a speeding car. As they approached the vehicle, they realized they had forgotten their ticket book back at the station. Muttering to themselves, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, they said, "Man, I'm such a fuck-up."

  2. Positive Intent: After maneuvering through heavy traffic, Officer Smith had successfully pulled over a speeding car. After driver left, and on their way back to their patrol car, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, they said to themselves, "That was a fucking good job."

  3. Neutral Intent: Following a routine traffic stop, after the driver left, Officer Smith walked back to his car, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, they said to themselves, "It's a fucking beautiful day."

Other-Directed (Colleague):

  1. Derogatory Intent: During a traffic stop, Officer Smith's partner, Officer Jones, accidentally dropped a piece of equipment. Irritated, Officer Smith, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, said, "Jones, you're a real fuck-up sometimes."

  2. Positive Intent: Officer Smith's partner, Officer Jones, skillfully diffused a potentially volatile situation during a traffic stop. Impressed, Officer Smith, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, said, "Jones, you handled that fucking brilliantly."

  3. Neutral Intent: Officer Smith was discussing the details of a traffic stop with Officer Jones. While describing the event, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, Officer Smith said, "The driver was going at least 20 miles over the fucking speed limit."

Other-Directed (Public):

  1. Derogatory Intent: Officer Smith had pulled over a driver who was being uncooperative. Speaking to the driver, Officer Smith, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, said, "Stop acting like a fucking idiot."

  2. Positive Intent: During a traffic stop, the driver immediately admitted to speeding and apologized. Appreciative of the driver's honesty, Officer Smith, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, said, "I fucking appreciate your honesty. It's refreshing."

  3. Neutral Intent: While explaining the reason for a traffic stop to a driver, Officer Smith, with their words recorded on their body-worn camera, said, "You were going over the fucking speed limit."

Balance Table

Appendix Table A1: Balance Table, by Target Treatment

Treatment Received

Characteristic

N

Self,

N = 1,741

Colleague,

N = 1,757

Public,

N = 1,782

p-value

Experience (Current)

5,268

10.51 (10.09)

10.74 (10.25)

10.72 (10.23)

>0.9

Experience (Total)

5,276

27.46 (9.69)

27.77 (9.89)

27.37 (10.29)

>0.9

Age

5,090

53.59 (8.54)

53.86 (8.91)

53.79 (8.98)

>0.9

Population

3,777

34,292.22 (115,185.22)

38,278.91 (135,859.31)

43,212.21 (164,147.11)

>0.9

Police Expenditures

3,777

6,076.80 (18,014.73)

6,869.19 (24,636.04)

7,474.22 (28,939.90)

>0.9

Fulltime Officers

3,777

38.18 (84.70)

43.46 (130.22)

46.18 (143.80)

>0.9

Education

5,094

>0.9

HS or equivalent

240 / 1,689 (14%)

240 / 1,699 (14%)

244 / 1,706 (14%)

Associate's

301 / 1,689 (18%)

296 / 1,699 (17%)

305 / 1,706 (18%)

Bachelor's

514 / 1,689 (30%)

539 / 1,699 (32%)

522 / 1,706 (31%)

Master's

576 / 1,689 (34%)

562 / 1,699 (33%)

570 / 1,706 (33%)

PhD, JD, MD, etc.

58 / 1,689 (3.4%)

62 / 1,699 (3.6%)

65 / 1,706 (3.8%)

Race

5,054

0.8

American Indian or Alaska Native

12 / 1,671 (0.7%)

6 / 1,689 (0.4%)

10 / 1,694 (0.6%)

Asian

4 / 1,671 (0.2%)

3 / 1,689 (0.2%)

5 / 1,694 (0.3%)

Black or African American

59 / 1,671 (3.5%)

70 / 1,689 (4.1%)

72 / 1,694 (4.3%)

Hispanic

46 / 1,671 (2.8%)

48 / 1,689 (2.8%)

54 / 1,694 (3.2%)

More than one race

50 / 1,671 (3.0%)

67 / 1,689 (4.0%)

55 / 1,694 (3.2%)

Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander

2 / 1,671 (0.1%)

1 / 1,689 (<0.1%)

1 / 1,694 (<0.1%)

White

1,498 / 1,671 (90%)

1,494 / 1,689 (88%)

1,497 / 1,694 (88%)

Sex

5,050

0.6

Male

1,380 / 1,673 (82%)

1,396 / 1,683 (83%)

1,382 / 1,694 (82%)

Female

293 / 1,673 (18%)

287 / 1,683 (17%)

312 / 1,694 (18%)

Role

5,118

>0.9

Head of HR

426 / 1,692 (25%)

432 / 1,697 (25%)

442 / 1,729 (26%)

Head of Law Enforcement

1,266 / 1,692 (75%)

1,265 / 1,697 (75%)

1,287 / 1,729 (74%)

Gov. Category

3,777

0.4

county

186 / 1,252 (15%)

191 / 1,258 (15%)

206 / 1,267 (16%)

municipality

888 / 1,252 (71%)

892 / 1,258 (71%)

911 / 1,267 (72%)

township

178 / 1,252 (14%)

175 / 1,258 (14%)

150 / 1,267 (12%)

Intent

5,280

0.5

Neutral

594 / 1,741 (34%)

594 / 1,757 (34%)

569 / 1,782 (32%)

Derogatory

566 / 1,741 (33%)

594 / 1,757 (34%)

593 / 1,782 (33%)

Positive

581 / 1,741 (33%)

569 / 1,757 (32%)

620 / 1,782 (35%)

Mean (SD); n / N (%)

Pearson's Chi-squared test

Main Experimental Results: Attentive Sample Only

As part of the design of this experiment, a quality check was incorporated to verify that participants were actively engaged with and correctly comprehending the presented material. The included attention check question, appearing after the four vignettes had been presented, and responses to each collected, directly references a detail provided in the vignettes: "From what you recall, how many complaints had been lodged against the officer in the first scenario you reviewed?" Possible responses include "none," "one," "two," or "unknown."

This question served as an indicator of participant attentiveness and comprehension, as the correct response ("none") is clearly stated in the vignette and prompt. It is expected that participants who are attentively reading and understanding the scenarios will answer correctly. If participants fail this check, it may suggest a lack of attention or misunderstanding of the material.

However, as seen in Appendix Table A2, when constraining the sample to only attentive respondents, the results do not change. This suggests that while approximately 18.8% of respondents failed the attentiveness check, this did not affect the main reported results.

Appendix Table A2: Mixed Effects Model, Main Effects (Attentive Only)

Appropriate

Professional

Public Trust

Discipline (Policy)

Discipline (Personal)

[Target] Colleague

-0.202 (0.023)***

-0.205 (0.021)***

-0.048

(0.019)*

0.144

(0.019)***

0.152

(0.019)***

[Target] Public

-1.127 (0.023)***

-1.034 (0.021)***

-0.709 (0.019)***

1.022

(0.019)***

1.075

(0.019)***

[Intent] Derogatory

-0.111 (0.023)***

-0.109 (0.021)***

0.000

(0.019)

0.102

(0.019)***

0.094

(0.020)***

[Intent] Positive

0.045

(0.023)*

0.044

(0.021)*

0.125

(0.019)***

-0.075 (0.019)***

-0.079 (0.019)***

Intercept

2.520

(0.025)***

2.369

(0.023)***

2.523

(0.022)***

1.552

(0.022)***

1.562

(0.023)***

SD (Intercept Respondent)

0.469

0.425

0.460

0.449

0.479

SD (Observations)

0.568

0.523

0.467

0.464

0.477

Num.Obs.

4152

4167

4153

4153

4170

R2 Marg.

0.310

0.309

0.199

0.333

0.334

R2 Cond.

0.590

0.584

0.594

0.655

0.668

AIC

8492.2

7811.2

7148.6

7068.7

7391.5

BIC

8536.5

7855.5

7192.9

7113.0

ICC

0.4

0.4

0.5

0.5

0.5

RMSE

0.51

0.47

0.42

0.42

0.43

Experimental Results with Controls

Appendix Table A3: Mixed Effects Model: Main Effects with Controls

Appropriate

Professional

Trust

Discipline Policy

Discipline Personal

[Target] Colleague

-0.247 (0.024)***

-0.234 (0.022)***

-0.077 (0.021)***

0.167

(0.021)***

0.166

(0.022)***

[Target] Public

-1.116 (0.024)***

-1.023 (0.022)***

-0.738 (0.021)***

1.044

(0.021)***

1.099

(0.022)***

[Intent] Derogatory

-0.140 (0.024)***

-0.123 (0.022)***

-0.002

(0.021)

0.120

(0.021)***

0.119

(0.022)***

[Intent] Positive

0.043

(0.024)+

0.056

(0.022)*

0.130

(0.021)***

-0.090 (0.021)***

-0.093 (0.022)***

Role: Law Enforcement

0.074

(0.046)

0.100

(0.041)*

-0.011

(0.045)

-0.004

(0.044)

-0.038

(0.046)

Career (Years)

-0.003

(0.002)+

-0.001

(0.002)

-0.005

(0.002)**

0.007

(0.002)***

0.007

(0.002)***

Fulltime Officers

0.000 (0.000)

0.000 (0.000)

0.000 (0.000)*

0.000 (0.000)

0.000 (0.000)

Education

0.049 (0.017)**

0.044 (0.015)**

0.051 (0.016)**

-0.043 (0.016)**

-0.048 (0.017)**

Race: Nonwhite

0.001 (0.058)

-0.029 (0.052)

-0.001 (0.057)

0.106 (0.056)+

0.112 (0.059)+

Sex: Female

-0.032 (0.050)

-0.054 (0.046)

-0.140 (0.050)**

-0.009 (0.049)

0.041 (0.051)

Intercept

2.440 (0.076)***

2.222 (0.069)***

2.568 (0.074)***

1.471 (0.073)***

1.499 (0.076)***

SD (Intercept Respondent)

0.451

0.403

0.463

0.450

0.475

SD (Observations)

0.553

0.513

0.480

0.481

0.497

Num.Obs.

3562

3577

3570

3562

3580

R2 Marg.

0.321

0.323

0.215

0.341

0.346

R2 Cond.

0.592

0.581

0.594

0.649

0.658

AIC

7140.6

6586.4

6372.8

6327.8

6619.9

BIC

7227.1

6672.9

6459.4

6414.3

6706.4

ICC

0.4

0.4

0.5

0.5

0.5

RMSE

0.50

0.46

0.43

0.43

0.45

Note: Comparison groups are Role: HR Executive, Race: White, Sex: Male

Tabular Main Interaction Results

Appendix Table A4: Mixed Effects Model, Main and Interactive Effects

Appropriate

Professional

Public Trust

Discipline (Policy)

Discipline (Personal)

[Target] Colleague

-0.102

(0.034)**

-0.135 (0.032)***

-0.067

(0.029)*

0.075

(0.029)**

0.053

(0.030)+

[Target] Public

-1.158 (0.034)***

-1.116 (0.032)***

-0.845 (0.029)***

1.103

(0.029)***

1.156

(0.030)***

[Intent] Derogatory

0.011

(0.034)

-0.030

(0.032)

0.049

(0.030)+

-0.038

(0.029)

-0.057

(0.030)+

[Intent] Positive

0.009

(0.034)

-0.032

(0.032)

-0.035

(0.029)

0.017

(0.029)

0.009

(0.030)

Colleague x Derogatory

-0.480 (0.049)***

-0.396 (0.045)***

-0.173 (0.042)***

0.356

(0.041)***

0.400

(0.043)***

Public x Derogatory

0.117

(0.049)*

0.161

(0.045)***

0.047

(0.042)

0.024

(0.041)

0.015

(0.043)

Colleague x Positive

0.114

(0.049)*

0.132

(0.045)**

0.182

(0.042)***

-0.083

(0.041)*

-0.061

(0.043)

Public x Positive

0.000

(0.048)

0.093

(0.045)*

0.292

(0.041)***

-0.228 (0.041)***

-0.227 (0.042)***

Intercept

2.469

(0.027)***

2.353

(0.025)***

2.542

(0.024)***

1.582

(0.024)***

1.609

(0.025)***

SD (Intercept Respondent)

0.475

0.434

0.477

0.464

0.498

SD (Observations)

0.550

0.511

0.468

0.465

0.479

Num.Obs.

5258

5270

5248

5246

5273

R2 Marg.

0.327

0.324

0.208

0.335

0.335

R2 Cond.

0.614

0.607

0.611

0.667

0.680

AIC

10531.7

9738.2

9162.9

9051.0

9494.2

BIC

10603.9

9810.5

9235.1

9123.2

9566.5

ICC

0.4

0.4

0.5

0.5

0.5

RMSE

0.49

0.46

0.42

0.41

0.43

Appendix Table A4a: Main versus Interaction Model Comparisons

Outcome Variable

N Params

AIC

BIC

LogLik

Deviance

Chi Squared

df

p-value

Appropriate (main)

7

10749

10795

-5367.4

10735

-

-

-

Appropriate (interaction)

11

10482

10555

-5230.3

10460

274.34

4

< 2.2e-16

Professional (main)

7

9925.4

9971.4

-4955.7

9911.4

-

-

-

Professional (interaction)

11

9687.7

9760

-4832.8

9665.7

245.74

4

< 2.2e-16

Trust (main)

7

9205.8

9251.8

-4595.9

9191.8

-

-

-

Trust (interaction)

11

9111.2

9183.4

-4544.6

9089.2

102.64

4

< 2.2e-16

Discipline Policy (main)

7

9144.4

9190.3

-4565.2

9130.4

-

-

-

Discipline Policy (interaction)

11

8999.1

9071.3

-4488.6

8977.1

153.26

4

< 2.2e-16

Discipline Personal (main)

7

9604.2

9650.2

-4795.1

9590.2

-

-

-

Discipline Personal (interaction)

11

9442.9

9515.2

-4710.5

9420.9

169.28

4

< 2.2e-16

Diversions from Pre-Registration

The only significant diversion from the study pre-registration and the published version here is that the original pre-registration called for a much larger sampling frame, to include not just law enforcement and human resources, but governing board member, head of communications, top appointed executive, top appointed official, and top elected official. In the end, this was decided to be too large a sample for a relatively narrowly framed experiment, and the sampling frame was reduced to just heads of law enforcement and human resources.

Pre-registered Hypotheses and Associated Findings

Table A5: Pre-registered Results

Pre-registered Hypothesis

Study Finding

Perceived Appropriateness

The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as more acceptable than when other-directed (colleague or public).

Confirmed through experiment.

Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as more acceptable than derogatory intent.

Confirmed through experiment

Perceived Professionalism

The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as more professional than when other-directed (colleague or public).

Confirmed through experiment

Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as more professional than derogatory intent.

Confirmed through experiment

Perceived Impact on Public Trust

The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as having less negative impact on public trust than when other-directed (colleague or public).

Confirmed through experiment

Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as having less negative impact on public trust than derogatory intent.

Partially confirmed. Compared to a neutral intent, derogatory intent is not seen as having a significantly different effect on public trust. This may signal police and HR executive distrust that the public can discern between neutral and derogatory intent. However, positive intent was seen as having less of a damaging effect compared to neutral intent.

Severity of Policy-Based Discipline

The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as warranting less severe policy-based disciplinary action than when other-directed (colleague or public).

Confirmed through experiment

Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as warranting less severe policy-based disciplinary action than derogatory intent.

Confirmed through experiment

Severity of Personal Opinion-Based Discipline

The use of profanity when self-directed or situation-directed will be perceived as warranting less severe personal opinion-based disciplinary action than when other-directed (colleague or public).

Confirmed through experiment

Positive or neutral intent in profanity use will be perceived as warranting less severe personal opinion-based disciplinary action than derogatory intent.

Confirmed through experiment

Comments
0
comment
No comments here
Why not start the discussion?